Selective neurophysiologic responses to music in instrumentalists with different listening biographies

To appropriately adapt to constant sensory stimulation, neurons in the auditory system are tuned to various acoustic characteristics, such as center frequencies, frequency modulations, and their combinations, particularly those combinations that carry species‐specific communicative functions. The present study asks whether such tunings extend beyond acoustic and communicative functions to auditory self‐relevance and expertise. More specifically, we examined the role of the listening biography—an individual's long term experience with a particular type of auditory input—on perceptual‐neural plasticity. Two groups of expert instrumentalists (violinists and flutists) listened to matched musical excerpts played on the two instruments (J.S. Bach Partitas for solo violin and flute) while their cerebral hemodynamic responses were measured using fMRI. Our experimental design allowed for a comprehensive investigation of the neurophysiology (cerebral hemodynamic responses as measured by fMRI) of auditory expertise (i.e., when violinists listened to violin music and when flutists listened to flute music) and nonexpertise (i.e., when subjects listened to music played on the other instrument). We found an extensive cerebral network of expertise, which implicates increased sensitivity to musical syntax (BA 44), timbre (auditory association cortex), and sound–motor interactions (precentral gyrus) when listening to music played on the instrument of expertise (the instrument for which subjects had a unique listening biography). These findings highlight auditory self‐relevance and expertise as a mechanism of perceptual‐neural plasticity, and implicate neural tuning that includes and extends beyond acoustic and communication‐relevant structures. Hum Brain Mapp 2009. © 2007 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  E. Donchin,et al.  Do Re Mi Fa Sol La Ti——Constraints, Congruity, and Musical Training: An Event-Related Brain Potentials Study of Musical Expectancies , 2002 .

[2]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Event-Related fMRI of the Auditory Cortex , 1998, NeuroImage.

[3]  A. Friederici,et al.  Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  Nobuo Suga,et al.  Sharpening of frequency tuning by inhibition in the central auditory system: tribute to Yasuji Katsuki , 1995, Neuroscience Research.

[5]  E. Saltzman,et al.  Action Representation of Sound: Audiomotor Recognition Network While Listening to Newly Acquired Actions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Mireille Besson,et al.  Brain waves associated with musical incongruities differ for musicians and non-musicians , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  S. Koelsch,et al.  Effects of musical expertise on the early right anterior negativity: an event-related brain potential study. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[8]  Gottfried Schlaug,et al.  The effect of musicianship on pitch memory in performance matched groups , 2003, Neuroreport.

[9]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction , 2006, NeuroImage.

[10]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  B. Ross,et al.  COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: Timbre-specific enhancement of auditory cortical representations in musicians , 2022 .

[12]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  Human pitch perception is reflected in the timing of stimulus-related cortical activity , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Jackendoff,et al.  A Generative Theory of Tonal Music , 1985 .

[14]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  Vesa Välimäki,et al.  Neural representations of the hierarchical scale pitch structure , 2007 .

[16]  Michael J. Martinez,et al.  The Role of the Insular Cortex in Pitch Pattern Perception: The Effect of Linguistic Contexts , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  E. T. Possing,et al.  Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  J. Rauschecker Cortical processing of complex sounds , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[19]  R. Miall,et al.  A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of others , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Tyler K. Perrachione,et al.  Neural characteristics of successful and less successful speech and word learning in adults , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[21]  Leonard B. Meyer Style and Music: Theory, History, and Ideology , 1989 .

[22]  H. Nusbaum,et al.  Neural Bases of Talker Normalization , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  E. Altenmüller,et al.  Specialization of the Specialized: Electrophysiological Investigations in Professional Musicians , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[25]  Jens Haueisen,et al.  Involuntary Motor Activity in Pianists Evoked by Music Perception , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  R. Kakigi,et al.  One year of musical training affects development of auditory cortical-evoked fields in young children. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  R. Bowtell,et al.  “sparse” temporal sampling in auditory fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[28]  T. Ohnishi,et al.  Functional anatomy of musical perception in musicians , 2001, NeuroImage.

[29]  Seth J. Gillihan,et al.  Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  N Suga,et al.  The corticofugal system for hearing: recent progress. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  M. Besson,et al.  AN EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERP) STUDY OF MUSICAL EXPECTANCY : COMPARISON OF MUSICIANS WITH NONMUSICIANS , 1995 .

[32]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Music, the food of neuroscience? , 2005, Nature.

[34]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Event-related fMRI of the auditory cortex. , 1998, NeuroImage.

[35]  A. Friederici,et al.  Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  W. O'Neill,et al.  An extralemniscal component of the mustached bat inferior colliculus selective for direction and rate of linear frequency modulations , 2000, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[37]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. , 1995, Science.

[38]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  S. Scott,et al.  Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  M Hoke,et al.  Tonotopic organization of the auditory cortex: pitch versus frequency representation. , 1989, Science.

[41]  C. Palmer Music performance. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[42]  David A. Medler,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi040 Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published February 9, 2005 , 2022 .

[43]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Varieties of musical experience , 2006, Cognition.

[44]  Nicole M. Russo,et al.  Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.