Top-down Mechanisms for Working Memory and Attentional Processes

Acknowledgements We wish to thank David Sturman for assistance in the preparation of the manuscript. Introduction Maintaining information in working memory (WM), such as when we rehearse the route to a restaurant, as well as directing attention, such as when we search for a familiar face in the crowd, constitute fundamental cognitive capabilities of the primate brain. Both activities are considered " top-down " , as they depend on goal-directed processes that rely on previous knowledge, and not incoming sensory stimulation. Top-down processes should be contrasted to " bottom-up " ones, such as the deployment of attention in reaction to a sudden movement in the periphery of the visual field, which is a sensory-driven, reflexive process. Understanding the neural bases of top-down control involved in WM and visual attention, is among the most challenging goals of cognitive neuroscience. In this chapter, we will review neuroimaging studies that have investigated this question in the past decade or so. The picture emerging from this line of research points to considerable overlap between the neural substrates that control WM and those that control attention. At the same time, there is growing evidence that the control structures for attention differ according to the type of attentive process, in that goal-directed and reflexive attention appear to be mediated by separable networks of frontal and parietal regions.

[1]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Subcortical connections of inferior temporal areas TE and TEO in macaque monkeys , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[2]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for mnemonic "scotomas" , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Cortical connections of inferior temporal area TEO in macaque monkeys , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[4]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Pulvinar lesions in monkeys produce abnormal scanning of a complex visual array , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Working Memory , 2001 .

[6]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[7]  S E Petersen,et al.  A positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of verbal information , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  Eric Courchesne,et al.  ERP Evidence for a Shifting Attention Deficit in Patients with Damage to the Cerebellum , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. , 1998, Science.

[10]  J M Fuster,et al.  Neuronal firing in the inferotemporal cortex of the monkey in a visual memory task , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  C D Frith,et al.  Space-based and object-based visual attention: shared and specific neural domains. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  P. Goldman-Rakic Cellular basis of working memory , 1995, Neuron.

[13]  R. Passingham,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[14]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Prefrontal unit activity during delayed conditional Go/No-go discrimination in the monkey. I. Relation to the stimulus , 1986, Brain Research.

[15]  Joaquin M. Fuster,et al.  Single cell activity in ventral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys , 1981, Brain Research.

[16]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural Correlates of Visual Working Memory fMRI Amplitude Predicts Task Performance , 2002, Neuron.

[17]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  D. LaBerge,et al.  Positron emission tomographic measurements of pulvinar activity during an attention task , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Attentional control of the processing of neural and emotional stimuli. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[21]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[22]  Edward Awh,et al.  Spatial versus Object Working Memory: PET Investigations , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[24]  H. Karnath,et al.  The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Areas Involved in Encoding and Applying Directional Expectations to Moving Objects , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Sustained Activity in the Medial Wall during Working Memory Delays , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[28]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  P. Strick,et al.  Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. , 1994, Science.

[30]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Schmahmann From movement to thought: Anatomic substrates of the cerebellar contribution to cognitive processing , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[32]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  B Giesbrecht,et al.  Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[34]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. , 1970, Experimental neurology.

[35]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes , 1997 .

[36]  A. Nobre,et al.  The Large-Scale Neural Network for Spatial Attention Displays Multifunctional Overlap But Differential Asymmetry , 1999, NeuroImage.

[37]  H. Niki,et al.  Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation performance in monkeys. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory , 1997, Nature.

[39]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Connections of inferior temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex in macaque monkeys. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  L. Chalupa,et al.  A review of cat and monkey studies implicating the pulvinar in visual function. , 1977, Behavioral biology.

[41]  W. T. Thach Motor Learning and Synaptic Plasticity in the Cerebellum: On the specific role of the cerebellum in motor learning and cognition: Clues from PET activation and lesion studies in man , 1997 .

[42]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Location- or Feature-Based Targeting of Peripheral Attention , 2001, NeuroImage.

[45]  G A Orban,et al.  Attention to One or Two Features in Left or Right Visual Field: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  J. Fuster,et al.  Delayed-matching and delayed-response deficit from cooling dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys. , 1976, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[47]  S. Ferber,et al.  Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.

[48]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[49]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Matching patterns of activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a spatial working memory task. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  S. Yantis,et al.  Mechanisms of attentional priority. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[54]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[55]  B. Postle,et al.  Activity in Human Frontal Cortex Associated with Spatial Working Memory and Saccadic Behavior , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Prefrontal unit activity during delayed conditional Go/No-go discrimination in the monkey. II. Relation to Go and No-go responses , 1986, Brain Research.

[58]  J. Fuster Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response performance: neuronal correlates of transient memory. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Inactivation of parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence of neural activity during memory-guided saccades. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[61]  E. Courchesne,et al.  A new role for the cerebellum in cognitive operations. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[62]  R. Passingham,et al.  Active maintenance in prefrontal area 46 creates distractor-resistant memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[63]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[64]  S. Petersen,et al.  Pulvinar nuclei of the behaving rhesus monkey: visual responses and their modulation. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  Ivan Toni,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[66]  J. Eastwood,et al.  Does unattended information facilitate change detection? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[67]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition , 2001 .

[68]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Projections from inferior temporal cortex to prefrontal cortex via the uncinate fascicle in rhesus monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[69]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  Stephen J. Boies,et al.  Components of attention. , 1971 .

[71]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[72]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[73]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update Time Is of the Essence , 2001, Neuron.

[74]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect , 2002 .

[75]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Neuron Activity Related to Short-Term Memory , 1971, Science.

[76]  S. Petersen,et al.  Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[77]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[78]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Role of Parietal Cortex in Verbal Working Memory , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[79]  B. Motter Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[80]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Attentional Activation of the Cerebellum Independent of Motor Involvement , 1997, Science.

[81]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[82]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET , 1993, Nature.

[83]  Elso Arruda,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: anatomy, physiology and neuropsychology of the frontal lobo , 1990 .