The order of disorder: Deconstructing visual disorder and its effect on rule-breaking.

Disorderly environments are linked to disorderly behaviors. Broken windows theory (Wilson & Kelling, 1982), an influential theory of crime and rule-breaking, assumes that scene-level social disorder cues (e.g., litter, graffiti) cause people to reason that they can get away with breaking rules. But what if part of the story is not about such complex social reasoning? Recent research suggests that basic visual disorder cues may be sufficient to encourage complex rule-breaking behavior. To test this hypothesis, we first conducted a set of experiments (Experiments 1-3) in which we identified basic visual disorder cues that generalize across visual stimuli with a variety of semantic content. Our results revealed that spatial features (e.g., nonstraight edges, asymmetry) are more important than color features (e.g., hue, saturation, value) for visual disorder. Exploiting this knowledge, we then reconstructed stimuli contrasted in terms of visual disorder, but absent of scene-level social disorder cues, to test whether visual disorder alone encourages cheating in a second set of experiments (Experiments 4 and 5). In these experiments, manipulating visual disorder increased the likelihood of cheating by up to 35% and the average magnitude of cheating by up to 87%. This work suggests that theories of rule-breaking that assume that complex social reasoning (e.g., about norms, policing, poverty) is necessary, should be reconsidered (e.g., Kelling & Coles, 1997; Sampson & Raudenbush, 2004). Furthermore, these experiments show that simple perceptual properties of the environment can affect complex behavior and sheds light on the extent to which our actions are within our control. (PsycINFO Database Record

[1]  Anthony A. Braga,et al.  POLICING CRIME AND DISORDER HOT SPOTS: A RANDOMIZED CONTROLLED TRIAL* , 2008 .

[2]  R. Blake,et al.  Negligible fronto-parietal BOLD activity accompanying unreportable switches in bistable perception , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  N. Schwarz Metacognitive Experiences in Consumer Judgment and Decision Making , 2004 .

[4]  Rebecca G. Boswell,et al.  Self-Regulatory Depletion Enhances Neural Responses to Rewards and Impairs Top-Down Control , 2013, Psychological science.

[5]  Catherine E. Ross,et al.  A new look at urban alienation : The effect of neighborhood disorder on perceived powerlessness , 1998 .

[6]  J. Jonides,et al.  The Cognitive Benefits of Interacting With Nature , 2008, Psychological science.

[7]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Building the gist of a scene: the role of global image features in recognition. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[8]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Network analysis of cortical visual pathways mapped with PET , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  Arie W Kruglanski,et al.  Leading us not unto temptation: momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  B. Zuckerman,et al.  A mediational model for the impact of exposure to community violence on early child behavior problems. , 2001, Child development.

[12]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[13]  Csongor Hajdu On The (honest) truth about dishonesty. how we lie to everyone – especially ourselves by Dan Ariely (2013, London, Harpercollins) , 2016 .

[14]  Boyoun Chae,et al.  Environmental Disorder Leads to Self-Regulatory Failure , 2014 .

[15]  J. Fleiss,et al.  Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Br Ferrall,et al.  Illusion of order: The false promise of broken windows policing , 2001 .

[18]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Grigori Yourganov,et al.  Observers' cognitive states modulate how visual inputs relate to gaze control. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Alexa M Tullett,et al.  Randomness increases self-reported anxiety and neurophysiological correlates of performance monitoring. , 2015, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[21]  D Marr,et al.  Theory of edge detection , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[22]  D. Casasanto,et al.  Spatial language and abstract concepts. , 2014, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[23]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Working memory and self-regulation , 2011 .

[24]  E. J. Williams The Comparison of Regression Variables , 1959 .

[25]  D. Cicchetti Guidelines, Criteria, and Rules of Thumb for Evaluating Normed and Standardized Assessment Instruments in Psychology. , 1994 .

[26]  Ryan Rahinel,et al.  Physical Order Produces Healthy Choices, Generosity, and Conventionality, Whereas Disorder Produces Creativity , 2013, Psychological science.

[27]  Anthony A. Braga,et al.  Problem-oriented policing in violent crime places: A randomized controlled experiment , 1999 .

[28]  Yoella Bereby-Meyer,et al.  Honesty Requires Time (and Lack of Justifications) , 2012, Psychological science.

[29]  W. Hofmann,et al.  How Depletion Operates in an Integrative Theory of Self-Control , 2016 .

[30]  Wesley G. Skogan,et al.  Fixing Broken Windows: Restoring Order and Reducing Crime in Our Communities by George L. Kelling and Catherine M. Coles:Life in the Gang: Family, Friends, and Violence , 1997 .

[31]  Hannah M. Hicks The (Honest) Truth About Dishonesty: How We Lie to Everyone—Especially Ourselves , 2015 .

[32]  D. Russell,et al.  Direct and moderating effects of community context on the psychological well-being of African American women. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Top-down influences on visual processing , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  R. A. Kinchla,et al.  The role of structural redundancy in the perception of visual targets , 1977 .

[35]  Catherine E. Ross,et al.  Neighborhood disadvantage and adult depression , 2000 .

[36]  Ralph B. Taylor,et al.  Ecological assessments of community disorder: Their relationship to fear of crime and theoretical implications , 1996, American journal of community psychology.

[37]  Dirk B. Walther,et al.  Natural Scene Categories Revealed in Distributed Patterns of Activity in the Human Brain , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  S. Raudenbush,et al.  Seeing Disorder: Neighborhood Stigma and the Social Construction of “Broken Windows” , 2004 .

[39]  Michael R. Gottfredson,et al.  A general theory of crime. , 1992 .

[40]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[41]  J. Bargh What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. , 2006, European journal of social psychology.

[42]  Nina Mazar,et al.  The Dishonesty of Honest People: A Theory of Self-Concept Maintenance , 2008 .

[43]  Grigori Yourganov,et al.  The Perception of Naturalness Correlates with Low-Level Visual Features of Environmental Scenes , 2014, PloS one.

[44]  M. Berman,et al.  Directed Attention as a Common Resource for Executive Functioning and Self-Regulation , 2010, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[45]  N. Schwarz,et al.  Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience? , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[46]  David J. Field,et al.  Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.

[47]  D. Molden Understanding Priming Effects in Social Psychology: What is “Social Priming” and How does it Occur? , 2014 .

[48]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Uniting the Tribes of Fluency to Form a Metacognitive Nation , 2009, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[49]  J. Wilson,et al.  BROKEN WINDOWS: THE POLICE AND NEIGHBOURHOOD SAFETY , 1982 .

[50]  W. Geisler Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural scenes. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[51]  A. Witkin,et al.  On the Role of Structure in Vision , 1983 .

[52]  Broken windows , 2018, The Routledge Companion to Criminological Theory and Concepts.

[53]  Philipp Berens,et al.  CircStat: AMATLABToolbox for Circular Statistics , 2009, Journal of Statistical Software.

[54]  D Marr,et al.  Early processing of visual information. , 1976, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[55]  Marc G. Berman,et al.  Can the High-Level Semantics of a Scene be Preserved in the Low-Level Visual Features of that Scene? A Study of Disorder and Naturalness , 2016, CogSci.

[56]  W. Hofmann,et al.  On Integrating the Components of Self-Control , 2015, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[57]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Overcoming intuition: metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[58]  D J Field,et al.  Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[59]  Linda Steg,et al.  The Spreading of Disorder , 2008, Science.

[60]  David G. Rand,et al.  The online laboratory: conducting experiments in a real labor market , 2010, ArXiv.

[61]  James V. Mahoney,et al.  Image Chunking: Defining Spatial Building Blocks for Scene Analysis , 1987 .

[62]  Hiroki Kotabe The world is random: a cognitive perspective on perceived disorder , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[63]  Dan Ariely,et al.  Unable to Resist Temptation: How Self-control Depletion Promotes Unethical Behavior , 2011 .

[64]  Michael C. Hout,et al.  Is the preference of natural versus man-made scenes driven by bottom–up processing of the visual features of nature? , 2015, Front. Psychol..