Dominance hierarchy and social grooming in female lion- tailed macaques (Macaca silenus) in the Western Ghats, India

This article reports the structure of dominance and its relationship with social grooming in wild lion-tailed macaque females. The strength of dominance hierarchy was 0.79 on a scale of 0 to 1 indicating a moderate linearity in the ranking system. Dominance scores were converted into an ordinal as well as an interval scale. Grooming scores were also converted into interval scales using standard scores. Grooming received and grooming given correlated positively and negatively respectively with dominance ranks indicating that high ranking females received more and gave less grooming. Grooming was also positively related to encounter rates for dyads of females. More grooming among adjacent ranks, and grooming being more reciprocal, occurred only in the case of dominant females. The grooming patterns, therefore, appeared to be more of despotic than egalitarian nature. While ranking macaques into different Grades of social systems ranging from despotic to egalitarian, Thierry (2004) has placed lion-tailed macaques in Grade 3 corresponding to the ‘relaxed’ social system. Our results indicate that the grooming and dominance relationships in this species are more despotic, and hence, the Grade for this species requires to be shifted toward 2 or 1.

[1]  Amit Kumar Sharma,et al.  Behavioural Responses of Lion-Tailed Macaques (Macaca silenus) to a Changing Habitat in a Tropical Rain Forest Fragment in the Western Ghats, India , 2001, Folia Primatologica.

[2]  B. Thierry,et al.  Covariation of Conflict Management Patterns across Macaque Species , 2001 .

[3]  Eric B. Keverne,et al.  Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships , 1989, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[4]  R. Terry Primate grooming as a tension reduction mechanism. , 1970, The Journal of psychology.

[5]  B. Thierry,et al.  Social grooming in Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) , 1990, International Journal of Primatology.

[6]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  The use of artificial-life models for the study of social organization , 2004 .

[7]  Carel P. van Schaik,et al.  The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates , 1997, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[8]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: Different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeys , 1989, American journal of primatology.

[9]  F. Waal Rank distance as a central feature of rhesus monkey social organization: a sociometric analysis , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  F. D. Waal The Chimpanzee's service economy: Food for grooming , 1997 .

[11]  Ajith T. T. Kumar The ecology and population dynamics of the lion-tailed macaque (Macaca silenus) in South India , 1987 .

[12]  F. Waal,et al.  The Integration of Dominance and Social Bonding in Primates , 1986, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[13]  H. G. Landau,et al.  On dominance relations and the structure of animal societies: II. Some effects of possible social factors , 1951 .

[14]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys , 1984, Nature.

[15]  D. Sade,et al.  Sociometrics of Macaca mulatta. I. Linkages and cliques in grooming matrices. , 1972, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[16]  Mewa Singh,et al.  Hierarchy, kinship and social interaction among Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) , 1992, Journal of Biosciences.

[17]  F. D. de Waal The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. , 1986, The Quarterly review of biology.

[18]  J. Manson Macaque societies: A model for the study of social organization , 2005 .

[19]  L. Barrett,et al.  The value of grooming to female primates , 2007, Primates.

[20]  P. Hammerstein,et al.  Biological markets. , 1995, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[21]  I. Tanaka,et al.  Elimination of External Parasites (Lice) Is the Primary Function of Grooming in Free-ranging Japanese Macaques , 1993 .

[22]  I. Bernstein,et al.  Macaque Societies. A Model for the Study of Social Organization , 2005, International Journal of Primatology.

[23]  Mewa Singh,et al.  Methodological considerations in measurement of dominance in primates , 2003 .

[24]  A. Estrada,et al.  Grooming down the hierarchy : Allogrooming in captive brown capuchin monkeys , 2008 .

[25]  L. D’souza,et al.  Inter- and intra-specific associations of Non-human Primates in Anaimalai Hills, South India , 1997 .

[26]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. , 1977, Journal of theoretical biology.

[27]  H. Landau On dominance relations and the structure of animal societies: I. Effect of inherent characteristics , 1951 .

[28]  B. Thierry,et al.  Patterns of agonistic interactions in three species of macaque (Macaca mulatta, M. fascicularis, M. tonkeana) , 1985 .

[29]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. , 1986, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[30]  H. Landau On dominance relations and the structure of animal societies: III The condition for a score structure , 1953 .

[31]  B. Chapais,et al.  Differential effects of kinship, dominance, and the mating season on female allogrooming in a captive group ofMacaca fuscata , 1988, Primates.

[32]  Amit Kumar Sharma,et al.  Distribution, population structure, and conservation of lion‐tailed macaques (Macaca silenus) in the Anaimalai Hills, Western Ghats, India , 2002, American journal of primatology.

[33]  L. Parr,et al.  Grooming down the hierarchy: allogrooming in captive brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[34]  J. Flack,et al.  Dominance Style, Social Power, and Conflict Management in Macaque Societies: A Conceptual Framework , 2003 .