Preverbal infants' understanding of referential gestures

In a short-term longitudinal study, 10- and 12-month-old infants' (N = 43) comprehension of referential gestures (gaze- and point- comprehension) was assessed independently in an attention- following task, and a preference-for-novelty task, measuring infants' encoding of actor-goal-relations (cf. Woodward 1998). Consistent with previous findings, marked developmental progress was found in gaze- and point-following between 10 and 12 months, with 12-month-olds approaching ceiling. In contrast, neither 10- nor 12-month-olds responded to a change in relation between actor and object, when the actor looked at or looked and pointed at the target object, whereas at 12 months of age they did, when the actor both looked at and reached for the target. These findings indicate that infants' ability to follow communicative gestures is not initially accompanied by an understanding of the intentional relation between communicator and referent independently of the communicative situation. The findings support the role of early pragmatic scaffolding in caretaker-infant interaction in building such an understanding.

[1]  Jon Driver,et al.  Adult's eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants , 1998 .

[2]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. , 1995 .

[3]  Julia Grant,et al.  The nonverbal context of mothers' speech to infants , 1983 .

[4]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  How infants make sense of intentional action , 2001 .

[5]  M. Tomasello The Role of Joint Attentional Processes in Early Language Development. , 1988 .

[6]  A. Pick,et al.  Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds' joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[7]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[8]  J. Townsend,et al.  Varieties of Binocular Interaction in Human Vision , 1998 .

[9]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[10]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. , 2001, Child development.

[11]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Interpersonal Understanding Fuels Knowledge Acquisition , 2000 .

[12]  D. Muir,et al.  Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. , 1996, Child development.

[13]  J. Bruner,et al.  Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language , 1978, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  Chris Moore Gaze following and the control of attention , 1999 .

[15]  H. Marcos,et al.  How adults contribute to the development of early referential communication? , 1991 .

[16]  C. Moore,et al.  Intentional relations and social understanding , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  G. Butterworth,et al.  Towards a Mechanism of Joint Visual Attention in Human Infancy , 1980 .

[18]  Intentional relations and triadic interactions , 1999 .

[19]  C. Moore,et al.  Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life , 1994 .

[20]  C. Moore,et al.  Sharing a perspective precedes the understanding of that perspective , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  Patricia Zukow-Goldring,et al.  Sensitive Caregiving Fosters the Comprehension of Speech: When Gestures Speak Louder than Words , 1996 .

[22]  A. Woodward Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors , 1999 .

[23]  S. E. Ulvund,et al.  The predictive validity of nonverbal communicative skills in infants with perinatal hazards , 1996 .

[24]  P. Morissette,et al.  Joint visual attention and pointing in infancy: A longitudinal study of comprehension , 1995 .

[25]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[26]  G. Butterworth,et al.  The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy : from perception to cognition , 1998 .

[27]  T. Saxon A longitudinal study of early mother-infant interaction and later language competence , 1997 .

[28]  J. Bruner Child's Talk: Learning to Use Language , 1985 .

[29]  G. Butterworth,et al.  Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy , 1996 .

[30]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Word Learning: A Window on Early Pragmatic Understanding , 1998 .

[31]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Understanding the link between joint attention and language. , 1995 .

[32]  C. Kasari,et al.  Joint attention and language gains in children with Down syndrome. , 1996, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[33]  C. Moore,et al.  The role of movement in the development of joint visual attention , 1997 .

[34]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[35]  G. Butterworth,et al.  What minds have in common is space : Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy , 1991 .

[36]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Reference: Intending that others jointly attend , 1998 .