Response-based strengthening in task shifting: evidence from shift effects produced by errors.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Patrick Rabbitt,et al. Processing a Display Even after you Make a Response to it. How Perceptual Errors can be Corrected , 1981, Cognitive Development and the Ageing Process.
[2] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[3] J. Driver,et al. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .
[4] B. J. Winer. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .
[5] Harold Pashler,et al. Is temporal spacing of tests helpful even when it inflates error rates? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[6] N. Meiran,et al. Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[7] Stefanie Schuch,et al. The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] T. Shallice,et al. Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .
[9] Patrick Rabbitt,et al. Consciousness is slower than you think , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[10] James L. McClelland. Failures to Learn and their Remediation: A Hebbian Account , 1999 .
[11] N. Meiran. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .
[12] E. D. Megaw,et al. Directional Errors and Their Correction in a Discrete Tracking Task , 1972 .
[13] E Ruthruff,et al. Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] R Hübner,et al. On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: evidence from two-component task shifts. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[15] M C Smith,et al. Repetition effect and short-term memory. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[16] M. Coles,et al. Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] R. Kliegl,et al. Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[18] H. Fairweather,et al. Choice reaction times in children: Error and post-error responses, and the repetition effect , 1978 .
[19] N. Meiran. Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.
[20] G. Logan,et al. Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] Stephen Monsell,et al. Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[22] M. Moscovitch,et al. Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .
[23] R De Jong,et al. An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .
[24] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[25] Bryan Rodgers,et al. What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .
[26] Ronald Hübner,et al. Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? , 2006, Psychological research.
[27] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.
[28] J R Anderson,et al. Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[29] U. Mayr. Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. , 2001, Psychology and aging.
[30] Darryl W. Schneider,et al. Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[31] P. Rabbitt. Time to detect errors as a function of factors affecting choice-response time. , 1967, Acta psychologica.
[32] D. Laming. Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .
[33] A. Allport,et al. Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.
[34] Aquiles Luna-Rodriguez,et al. Response selection difficulty and asymmetrical costs of switching between tasks and stimuli: no evidence for an exogenous component of task-set reconfiguration. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] B. Hommel,et al. Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[36] P Rabbitt,et al. Age, IQ and awareness, and recall of errors. , 1990, Ergonomics.
[37] Richard Ridderinkhof. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.
[38] T. Goschke. Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .
[39] I. Koch,et al. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] P M Rabbitt,et al. Three Kinds of Error-Signalling Responses in a Serial Choice Task , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[41] Marco Steinhauser,et al. Mixing costs in task shifting reflect sequential processing stages in a multicomponent task , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[42] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs , 2003 .
[43] David M. Riefer,et al. Multinomial processing models of source monitoring. , 1990 .
[44] M. Steinhauser,et al. Automatic activation of task-related representations in task shifting , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[45] I. Koch. The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[46] James L. McClelland,et al. Mechanisms of cognitive development : behavioral and neural perspectives , 2001 .
[47] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[48] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[49] Katja Fiehler,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of error correction. , 2005, Psychophysiology.
[50] D Laming,et al. Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.
[51] Erik M Altmann,et al. Repetition priming in task switching: Do the benefits dissipate? , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[52] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Modelling cognitive control in task switching and ageing , 2001 .
[53] Thomas Kleinsorge,et al. Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[54] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching , 2000 .
[55] S. Keele,et al. Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.