Response-based strengthening in task shifting: evidence from shift effects produced by errors.

The hypothesis is introduced that 1 source of shift costs is the strengthening of task-related associations occurring whenever an overt response is produced. The authors tested this account by examining shift effects following errors and error compensation processes. The authors predicted that following a specific type of error, called task confusion, shift benefits instead of shift costs should result. A series of 3 experiments confirmed this prediction showing that task confusions produce shift benefits in subsequent trials (Experiment 1), even when the error is detected (Experiment 2). Moreover, only posterror processes that imply an error correction response produce shift costs (Experiment 3). These results additionally suggest that error detection cannot prevent errors from affecting subsequent performance.

[1]  Patrick Rabbitt,et al.  Processing a Display Even after you Make a Response to it. How Perceptual Errors can be Corrected , 1981, Cognitive Development and the Ageing Process.

[2]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[3]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[4]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[5]  Harold Pashler,et al.  Is temporal spacing of tests helpful even when it inflates error rates? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  Stefanie Schuch,et al.  The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[9]  Patrick Rabbitt,et al.  Consciousness is slower than you think , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  James L. McClelland Failures to Learn and their Remediation: A Hebbian Account , 1999 .

[11]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[12]  E. D. Megaw,et al.  Directional Errors and Their Correction in a Discrete Tracking Task , 1972 .

[13]  E Ruthruff,et al.  Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  R Hübner,et al.  On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: evidence from two-component task shifts. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[15]  M C Smith,et al.  Repetition effect and short-term memory. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  M. Coles,et al.  Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  H. Fairweather,et al.  Choice reaction times in children: Error and post-error responses, and the repetition effect , 1978 .

[19]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[20]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[22]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .

[23]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[24]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[25]  Bryan Rodgers,et al.  What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .

[26]  Ronald Hübner,et al.  Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? , 2006, Psychological research.

[27]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[28]  J R Anderson,et al.  Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  U. Mayr Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[30]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[31]  P. Rabbitt Time to detect errors as a function of factors affecting choice-response time. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[32]  D. Laming Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .

[33]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[34]  Aquiles Luna-Rodriguez,et al.  Response selection difficulty and asymmetrical costs of switching between tasks and stimuli: no evidence for an exogenous component of task-set reconfiguration. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  P Rabbitt,et al.  Age, IQ and awareness, and recall of errors. , 1990, Ergonomics.

[37]  Richard Ridderinkhof Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.

[38]  T. Goschke Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .

[39]  I. Koch,et al.  The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  P M Rabbitt,et al.  Three Kinds of Error-Signalling Responses in a Serial Choice Task , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  Marco Steinhauser,et al.  Mixing costs in task shifting reflect sequential processing stages in a multicomponent task , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[42]  Joachim Hoffmann,et al.  Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs , 2003 .

[43]  David M. Riefer,et al.  Multinomial processing models of source monitoring. , 1990 .

[44]  M. Steinhauser,et al.  Automatic activation of task-related representations in task shifting , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[45]  I. Koch The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[46]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Mechanisms of cognitive development : behavioral and neural perspectives , 2001 .

[47]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[48]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[49]  Katja Fiehler,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of error correction. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[50]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[51]  Erik M Altmann,et al.  Repetition priming in task switching: Do the benefits dissipate? , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[52]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Modelling cognitive control in task switching and ageing , 2001 .

[53]  Thomas Kleinsorge,et al.  Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[54]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching , 2000 .

[55]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.