Dopamine and Octopamine Differentiate between Aversive and Appetitive Olfactory Memories in Drosophila

The catecholamines play a major role in the regulation of behavior. Here we investigate, in the fly Drosophila melanogaster, the role of dopamine and octopamine (the presumed arthropod homolog of norepinephrine) during the formation of appetitive and aversive olfactory memories. We find that for the formation of both types of memories, cAMP signaling is necessary and sufficient within the same subpopulation of mushroom-body intrinsic neurons. On the other hand, memory formation can be distinguished by the requirement for different catecholamines, dopamine for aversive and octopamine for appetitive conditioning. Our results suggest that in associative conditioning, different memories are formed of the same odor under different circumstances, and that they are linked to the respective motivational systems by their specific modulatory pathways.

[1]  W. Quinn,et al.  Reward learning in normal and mutant Drosophila. , 1983, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Mutations in the dopa decarboxylase gene affect learning in Drosophila. , 1984, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  A Borst,et al.  Drosophila mushroom body mutants are deficient in olfactory learning. , 1985, Journal of neurogenetics.

[4]  T. Tully Measuring learning in individual flies is not necessary to study the effects of single-gene mutations inDrosophila: A reply to Holliday and Hirsch , 1986, Behavior genetics.

[5]  Tim Tully,et al.  Drosophila learning and memory revisited , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  Y. Zhong,et al.  Altered synaptic plasticity in Drosophila memory mutants with a defective cyclic AMP cascade. , 1991, Science.

[7]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. , 1994, Science.

[8]  W. Quinn,et al.  A neuropeptide gene defined by the Drosophila memory mutant amnesiac. , 1995, Science.

[9]  W. Schultz,et al.  Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli , 1996, Nature.

[10]  T. Tully,et al.  CREB and the formation of long-term memory , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[11]  Tim Tully,et al.  Associative Learning Disrupted by Impaired Gs Signaling in Drosophila Mushroom Bodies , 1996, Science.

[12]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Conditioned visual flight orientation in Drosophila: dependence on age, practice, and diet. , 1996, Learning & memory.

[13]  M. Monastirioti,et al.  Characterization of Drosophila Tyramine β-HydroxylaseGene and Isolation of Mutant Flies Lacking Octopamine , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  DAMB, a Novel Dopamine Receptor Expressed Specifically in Drosophila Mushroom Bodies , 1996, Neuron.

[15]  Kei Ito,et al.  GAL4-responsive UAS-tau as a tool for studying the anatomy and development of the Drosophila central nervous system , 1997, Cell and Tissue Research.

[16]  M. Hammer,et al.  Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[17]  Scott T. Wong,et al.  Type I Adenylyl Cyclase Mutant Mice Have Impaired Mossy Fiber Long-Term Potentiation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  John H Byrne,et al.  New Perspectives on Classical Conditioning: a Synthesis of Hebbian and Non-Hebbian Mechanisms , 1998, Neuron.

[19]  K. Han,et al.  A Novel Octopamine Receptor with Preferential Expression inDrosophila Mushroom Bodies , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  R. Davis,et al.  Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markers. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[21]  R. Menzel,et al.  Pharmacological dissociation between the reinforcing, sensitizing, and response-releasing functions of reward in honeybee classical conditioning. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  K. Siwicki,et al.  Mushroom Body Ablation Impairs Short-Term Memory and Long-Term Memory of Courtship Conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster , 1999, Neuron.

[23]  W. Quinn,et al.  The amnesiac Gene Product Is Expressed in Two Neurons in the Drosophila Brain that Are Critical for Memory , 2000, Cell.

[24]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Tissue-specific expression of a type I adenylyl cyclase rescues the rutabaga mutant memory defect: in search of the engram. , 2000, Learning & memory.

[25]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. , 2000, Science.

[26]  E. Kandel The Molecular Biology of Memory Storage: A Dialogue Between Genes and Synapses , 2001, Science.

[27]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Molecular biology and anatomy of Drosophila olfactory associative learning , 2001, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[28]  R. Davis,et al.  The Role of Drosophila Mushroom Body Signaling in Olfactory Memory , 2001, Science.

[29]  Tim Tully,et al.  Disruption of neurotransmission in Drosophila mushroom body blocks retrieval but not acquisition of memory , 2001, Nature.

[30]  T. Kitamoto Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. , 2001, Journal of neurobiology.

[31]  Gene E. Robinson,et al.  Octopamine influences division of labor in honey bee colonies , 2001, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[32]  Michael T. Mader,et al.  The Drosophila Standard Brain , 2002, Current Biology.

[33]  Frederic Mery,et al.  Experimental evolution of learning ability in fruit flies , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  Martin Heisenberg,et al.  Extinction Antagonizes Olfactory Memory at the Subcellular Level , 2002, Neuron.

[35]  A. Barron,et al.  Octopamine modulates responsiveness to foraging-related stimuli in honey bees (Apis mellifera) , 2002, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[36]  I. Meinertzhagen,et al.  Synaptic organization of the mushroom body calyx in Drosophila melanogaster , 2002, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[37]  A. Komatsu,et al.  A trace amine, tyramine, functions as a neuromodulator in Drosophila melanogaster , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[38]  Jay Hirsh,et al.  Targeted gene expression in Drosophila dopaminergic cells using regulatory sequences from tyrosine hydroxylase. , 2003, Journal of neurobiology.

[39]  E. Kandel,et al.  Activity-Dependent Presynaptic Facilitation and Hebbian LTP Are Both Required and Interact during Classical Conditioning in Aplysia , 2003, Neuron.

[40]  K. Han,et al.  Expression of a D1 dopamine receptor dDA1/DmDOP1 in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogaster. , 2003, Gene expression patterns : GEP.

[41]  R. E. Page,et al.  Effect of pheromones, hormones, and handling on sucrose response thresholds of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) , 2003, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[42]  M. Heisenberg Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  E. Kandel The Molecular Biology of Memory Storage: A Dialog Between Genes and Synapses , 2004, Bioscience reports.

[44]  Y. Dudai,et al.  What is the possible contribution of Ca2+-stimulated adenylate cyclase to acquisition, consolidation and retention of an associative olfactory memory inDrosophila , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[45]  W. Quinn,et al.  Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutantDrosophila melanogaster , 1985, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[46]  M. Low,et al.  Disruption of neurotransmission in Drosophila mushroom body blocks retrieval but not acquisition of memory , 2022 .