The Emergence of Words: Attentional Learning in Form and Meaning

Children improve at word learning during the 2nd year of life-sometimes dramatically. This fact has suggested a change in mechanism, from associative learning to a more referential form of learning. This article presents an associative exemplar-based model that accounts for the improvement without a change in mechanism. It provides a unified account of children's growing abilities to (a) learn a new word given only 1 or a few training trials ("fast mapping"); (b) acquire words that differ only slightly in phonological form; (c) generalize word meanings preferentially along particular dimensions, such as object shape (the "shape bias"); and (d) learn 2nd labels for already-named objects, despite a persisting resistance to doing so ("mutual exclusivity"). The model explains these improvements in terms of increased attention to relevant aspects of form and meaning, which reduces memory interference. The interaction of associations and reference in word learning is discussed.

[1]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[2]  Linda B. Smith Learning How to Learn Words , 2000 .

[3]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Handbook of East Asian Psycholinguistics: The competition model , 2006 .

[6]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[7]  Chris Sinha,et al.  Symbol Grounding or the Emergence of Symbols? Vocabulary Growth in Children and a Connectionist Net , 1992 .

[8]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  A Curvilinear Trend in Naming Errors as a Function of Early Vocabulary Growth , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  M. Casasola,et al.  Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[10]  Joan L. Bybee,et al.  Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure , 2001 .

[11]  G. Miller,et al.  Linguistic theory and psychological reality , 1982 .

[12]  A. Woodward,et al.  Infants' learning about words and sounds in relation to objects. , 1999, Child development.

[13]  Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al.  Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast , 2000 .

[14]  D. Mareschal,et al.  Modeling Infant Speech Sound Discrimination Using Simple Associative Networks. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[15]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[16]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? , 1996, Cognition.

[17]  B. Campbell,et al.  Punishment and aversive behavior , 1969 .

[18]  Roberta Michnick Golinkoff,et al.  Becoming a word learner : a debate on lexical acquisition , 2000 .

[19]  C. Mervis,et al.  Two-year-olds readily learn multiple labels for the same basic-level category. , 1994, Child development.

[20]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[21]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .

[22]  W. Merriman,et al.  The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[23]  Douglas L. T. Rohde,et al.  Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small? , 1999, Cognition.

[24]  E. Markman,et al.  Word learning in children: an examination of fast mapping. , 1987, Child development.

[25]  Terry Regier,et al.  The Human Semantic Potential: Spatial Language and Constrained Connectionism , 1996 .

[26]  Carolyn Rovee-Collier,et al.  Bidirectional priming in infants , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[27]  C. Fillmore An Alternative to Checklist Theories of Meaning , 1975 .

[28]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[29]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. , 1992 .

[30]  Igor Farkaš,et al.  Modeling the Development of Lexicon with DevLex: A Self-Organizing Neural Network Model of Lexical Acquisition , 2019, Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[31]  L. Markson,et al.  Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children , 1997, Nature.

[32]  L. Kamin Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning , 1967 .

[33]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Catastrophic Interference in Connectionist Networks: The Sequential Learning Problem , 1989 .

[34]  C. Dollaghan,et al.  Fast mapping in normal and language-impaired children. , 1987, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[35]  N. Mackintosh SELECTIVE ATTENTION IN ANIMAL DISCRIMINATION LEARNING. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts , 2000, Cognition.

[37]  J. Aitchinson The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[38]  K Plunkett,et al.  Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions. , 1998, Child development.

[39]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[40]  J. Pearce A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. , 1987, Psychological review.

[41]  R. Aslin,et al.  Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children , 2000, Cognition.

[42]  E. Kleinknecht,et al.  Beyond fast mapping: young children's extensions of novel words and novel facts. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[43]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. , 1996, Child development.

[44]  T. Regier Emergent constraints on word-learning: a computational perspective , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  Sourabh Niyogi Bayesian Learning at the Syntax-Semantics Interface , 2002 .

[46]  M. Brent,et al.  The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development , 2001, Cognition.

[47]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .

[48]  Nadine Martin,et al.  Phonological Facilitation of Semantic Errors in Normal and Aphasic Speakers , 1996 .

[49]  J. Pearce Similarity and discrimination: a selective review and a connectionist model. , 1994, Psychological review.

[50]  J. Huttenlocher The origins of language comprehension. , 1974 .

[51]  V. Marchman,et al.  U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: Implications for child language acquisition , 1991, Cognition.

[52]  J. Elman Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.

[53]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[54]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[55]  G. Miller,et al.  Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.

[56]  M. Brent Advances in the computational study of language acquisition , 1996, Cognition.

[57]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  L. Gleitman The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.

[59]  Ping Li,et al.  A self-organizing neural network model of the acquisition of word meaning , 2001 .

[60]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Competition and Lexical Categorization , 1987 .

[61]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object Shape, Object Function, and Object Name , 1998 .

[62]  S. Waxman,et al.  Word learning is ‘smart’: evidence that conceptual information affects preschoolers' extension of novel words , 2002, Cognition.

[63]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The Domestication of Social Cognition in Dogs , 2002, Science.

[64]  R. Miikkulainen Dyslexic and Category-Specific Aphasic Impairments in a Self-Organizing Feature Map Model of the Lexicon , 1997, Brain and Language.

[65]  J. Kruschke,et al.  ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.

[66]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[67]  L. Markson,et al.  Children's Reliance on Creator's Intent in Extending Names for Artifacts , 2003, Psychological science.

[68]  J. Werker,et al.  Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks , 1997, Nature.

[69]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning internal representations by error propagation , 1986 .

[70]  G. Bower,et al.  From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[71]  Raymond J. Mooney,et al.  Acquiring Word-Meaning Mappings for Natural Language Interfaces , 2011, J. Artif. Intell. Res..

[72]  R. Aslin,et al.  Lexical Neighborhoods and the Word-Form Representations of 14-Month-Olds , 2002, Psychological science.

[73]  Michael R Brent,et al.  Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[74]  Michael Gasser,et al.  Learning Nouns and Adjectives: A Connectionist Account , 1998 .

[75]  Jeffrey Mark Siskind,et al.  Naive physics, event perception, lexical semantics, and language acquisition , 1992 .

[76]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition , 2001 .

[77]  Garrison W. Cottrell,et al.  Acquiring the Mapping from Meaning to Sounds , 1994, Connect. Sci..

[78]  S. Rose,et al.  Long-term memory for a spatial task in young chicks , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[79]  Chen Yu,et al.  The Role of Embodied Intention in Early Lexical Acquisition , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[80]  Todd M. Bailey,et al.  Phonological specificity in early words , 2002 .

[81]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Functionalism and the competition model , 1989 .

[82]  E. Dromi Early Lexical Development , 1987 .

[83]  J. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .

[84]  E. Clark Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition , 1997, Cognition.

[85]  Carla E. Brodley,et al.  Feature Selection for Unsupervised Learning , 2004, J. Mach. Learn. Res..

[86]  Alex Pentland,et al.  Learning words from sights and sounds: a computational model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[87]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[88]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Vocabulary Acquisition and Verbal Short-Term Memory: Computational and Neural Bases , 1997, Brain and Language.

[89]  J. Call,et al.  Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for "Fast Mapping" , 2004, Science.

[90]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  How infants make sense of intentional action , 2001 .

[91]  Daniel Jurafsky,et al.  The role of the lemma in form variation , 2002 .

[92]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Word learning as Bayesian inference. , 2007, Psychological review.

[93]  M. Laska,et al.  Food avoidance learning in squirrel monkeys and common marmosets. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[94]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .

[95]  Susan J. Hespos,et al.  Conceptual precursors to language , 2004, Nature.

[96]  E. Markman,et al.  Rapid Word Learning in 13- and 18-Month-Olds. , 1994 .

[97]  S. Goldinger Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. , 1998, Psychological review.

[98]  Lori Markson,et al.  Are there principles that apply only to the acquisition of words? A reply to Waxman and Booth , 2001, Cognition.

[99]  A G Barto,et al.  Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. , 1981, Psychological review.

[100]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Variability in early communicative development. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[101]  A. Gelperin,et al.  One-trial associative learning modifies food odor preferences of a terrestrial mollusc. , 1981, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[102]  Cynthia Fisher,et al.  On the semantic content of subcategorization frames , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[103]  David K. Dickinson,et al.  First impressions: Children’s knowledge of words gained from a single exposure , 1984, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[104]  P. Bloom How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .

[105]  E. Markman,et al.  Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning , 1994 .

[106]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Five questions for any theory of word learning , 2000 .

[107]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  Phonological specificity in children at 1;2 , 2005, Journal of Child Language.

[108]  B. Ripley,et al.  Pattern Recognition , 1968, Nature.

[109]  L. Smith,et al.  A model of perceptual classification in children and adults. , 1989, Psychological review.

[110]  J. Siskind A computational study of cross-situational techniques for learning word-to-meaning mappings , 1996, Cognition.

[111]  J. Werker,et al.  Infants' Ability to Learn Phonetically Similar Words: Effects of Age and Vocabulary Size , 2002 .

[112]  E. Ross Association , 1886, American Journal of Sociology.

[113]  J. Lucy,et al.  Grammatical categories and cognition: References , 1992 .

[114]  David C. Plaut,et al.  A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: extension to sequential processing , 1999, Cogn. Sci..