Gaze modulation of visual aftereffects

Physiological studies of non-human primates have suggested that the direction of gaze can modulate the gain of neuronal responses to visual stimuli in many cortical areas including V1. The neural gaze modulation is suggested to subserve the conversion from gaze-independent (eye-centered) to dependent (e.g., head-centered) representations. However, it has not been established whether the gaze modulation has significant influences on human visual perception. Here we show that gaze direction modestly but significantly modulates the magnitudes of the motion aftereffect, the tilt aftereffect and the size aftereffect. These aftereffects were stronger when the adaptation and test patterns were presented in the same gaze direction, than when they were presented in different gaze directions, even though the patterns always stimulated the same retinal location. The gaze modulation effect was not statistically significant for the post-adaptation elevation of contrast detection thresholds. The gaze modulation of visual aftereffects provides a useful psychophysical tool to analyze human cortical processes for coordinate transformations of visual space.

[1]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  M F Bradshaw,et al.  Vertical disparities, differential perspective and binocular stereopsis , 1993, Nature.

[3]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[4]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7a of macaque , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  Isamu Motoyoshi,et al.  Is the size aftereffect direction selective? , 1999, Vision Research.

[6]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[7]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Aftereffect of Seen Motion with a Stabilized Retinal Image , 1963, Science.

[8]  C. McCollough,et al.  THE CONDITIONING OF COLOR-PERCEPTION. , 1965, The American journal of psychology.

[9]  R. Andersen,et al.  The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light- sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. II. Differentiation of retinal from extraretinal inputs. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[12]  M. Graziano,et al.  Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  ANDREW SMITH,et al.  Tilt aftereffects with subjective contours , 1975, Nature.

[14]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Spatial vision. , 1971, Annual review of psychology.

[15]  J. Gibson,et al.  ADAPTATION , AFTEREFFECT AND CONTRAST IN THE PERCEPTION OF TILTED LINES , 2004 .

[16]  D. W. Heeley A perceived spatial frequency shift at orientations orthogonal to adapting gratings , 1979, Vision Research.

[17]  N. Sutherland Figural After-Effects and Apparent Size , 1961 .

[18]  D. Braun,et al.  Contingent aftereffects: Lateral interactions between color and motion , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  H. Barlow,et al.  Evidence for a Physiological Explanation of the Waterfall Phenomenon and Figural After-effects , 1963, Nature.

[20]  Hiroshi Ashida,et al.  A hierarchical structure of motion system revealed by interocular transfer of flicker motion aftereffects , 2000, Vision Research.

[21]  J. Mayhew After-effects of movement contingent on direction of gaze. , 1973, Vision Research.

[22]  R A Andersen,et al.  Influence of gaze rotation on the visual response of primate MSTd neurons. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[24]  S. Celebrini,et al.  Gaze direction controls response gain in primary visual-cortex neurons , 1999, Nature.

[25]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The orientation specificity of two visual after‐effects , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  A. Parker Shifts in perceived periodicity induced by temporal modulation and their influence on the spatial frequency tuning of two aftereffects , 1981, Vision Research.

[27]  J. Harris,et al.  Movement aftereffects contingent on the colour or pattern of a stationary surround , 1975, Vision Research.

[28]  Z Kourtzi,et al.  Representation of Perceived Object Shape by the Human Lateral Occipital Complex , 2001, Science.

[29]  Tutis Vilis,et al.  Eye position signals modulate early dorsal and ventral visual areas. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  S. Squatrito,et al.  Encoding of Smooth Pursuit Direction and Eye Position by Neurons of Area MSTd of Macaque Monkey , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[32]  C. McCollough Color Adaptation of Edge-Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1965, Science.

[33]  C Blakemore,et al.  On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[35]  J. Duin,et al.  On the relation of stereoacuity to interocular transfer of the motion and the tilt aftereffects , 1983, Vision Research.

[36]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  D. Heeger,et al.  Neuronal Basis of the Motion Aftereffect Reconsidered , 2001, Neuron.

[38]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Subjective contours, tilt aftereffects, and visual cortical organization , 1989, Vision Research.

[39]  C. Galletti,et al.  Gaze-dependent visual neurons in area V3A of monkey prestriate cortex , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[40]  I. Howard,et al.  Relative shear disparities and the perception of surface inclination , 1994, Vision Research.

[41]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Recovery of fMRI activation in motion area MT following storage of the motion aftereffect. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Pursuit and optokinetic deficits following chemical lesions of cortical areas MT and MST. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  A. Dale,et al.  Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, Nature.

[44]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Motion Aftereffect:A Modern Perspective , 1998 .

[45]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Dual multiple-scale processing for motion in the human visual System , 1997, Vision Research.

[46]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  A new view of hemineglect based on the response properties of parietal neurones. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[47]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  Physiological Studies of Motion Inputs. , 1994 .

[48]  S. Anstis,et al.  Separate motion aftereffects from each eye and from both eyes , 1983, Vision Research.

[49]  I KOHLER,et al.  Experiments with goggles. , 1962, Scientific American.

[50]  J. Frisby Seeing: Illusion, Brain and Mind , 1979 .

[51]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Size Adaptation: A New Aftereffect , 1969, Science.

[52]  Peter Wenderoth,et al.  Mechanisms of purely subjective contour tilt aftereffects , 1995, Vision Research.

[53]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[54]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Object Priming in Humans Revealed by Rapid Presentation Event-Related fMRI , 1998, Neuron.

[56]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons , 1988, Nature.