The Consumption of Breakfast, Fish and/or Caffeine does not Predict Study Progress in Adult Distance Education.

Consumption of breakfast, fish, or caffeine are each separately often investigated in relation to learning performance in traditional education, but not in distance education (DE). The objective of this study was to investigate whether the relations between the consumption of breakfast, fish, and/or caffeine on the one hand and learning performance on the other are also found in DE students. This population is different from traditional students and characterized by a different profile in terms of age, personal and work responsibilities as well as other demographics. In an observational longitudinal study, the consumption of breakfast, fish, and caffeine of 1157 DE students (18-76 years old, mean 35.8 ± 11.1 years) was used to predict learning performance using multiple regression analysis. In an online digital survey, university students provided information about their consumption of these nutritional measures and on important covariates. Learning performance, measured as study progress (i.e., the number of successfully completed modules), was evaluated objectively after 14 months. Results showed that adding the consumption of breakfast, fish, and caffeine to the covariate model did not fit the data better, χ2 (3, N = 1155) = 3.287, p = 0.35. This means that neither the consumption of breakfast nor fish nor caffeine predicted study progress in adults participating in DE. This study is important as it is the first to report on these relations in this specific age group and educational setting, which is increasingly important due to the increased preference for this type of education.

[1]  Paul A. Kirschner,et al.  The Adult Learning Open University Determinants (ALOUD) study: Biological and psychological factors associated with learning performance in adult distance education , 2015, Br. J. Educ. Technol..

[2]  D. Alexander,et al.  Docosahexaenoic Acid and Adult Memory: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis , 2015, PloS one.

[3]  Trends in student population and curriculum design for the Open University of the Netherlands , 2014 .

[4]  J. Owens,et al.  Effect of energy drink and caffeinated beverage consumption on sleep, mood, and performance in children and adolescents. , 2014, Nutrition reviews.

[5]  J. Owens Insufficient Sleep in Adolescents and Young Adults: An Update on Causes and Consequences , 2014, Pediatrics.

[6]  W. Stonehouse Does Consumption of LC Omega-3 PUFA Enhance Cognitive Performance in Healthy School-Aged Children and throughout Adulthood? Evidence from Clinical Trials , 2014, Nutrients.

[7]  P. Rogers Caffeine and Alertness: In Defense of Withdrawal Reversal , 2014 .

[8]  T. Zilberter,et al.  Breakfast and cognition: sixteen effects in nine populations, no single recipe , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[9]  Cai Song,et al.  Cognitive enhancement by omega-3 fatty acids from child-hood to old age: Findings from animal and clinical studies , 2013, Neuropharmacology.

[10]  I. Huybrechts,et al.  Mapping low intake of micronutrients across Europe , 2013, British Journal of Nutrition.

[11]  A. Diamond Executive functions. , 2014, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[12]  J. Bryan,et al.  Influences of micronutrient and omega-3 fatty acid supplementation on cognition, learning, and behavior: methodological considerations and implications for children and adolescents in developed societies. , 2012, Nutrition reviews.

[13]  P. Calder,et al.  Lower omega-3 fatty acid intake and status are associated with poorer cognitive function in older age: A comparison of individuals with and without cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease , 2012, Nutritional neuroscience.

[14]  Sarah A. Chau,et al.  Effects of omega-3 fatty acids on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[15]  J. Karr,et al.  Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and cognition throughout the lifespan: A review , 2011, Nutritional neuroscience.

[16]  I. Sigfusdottir,et al.  Adolescent substance use, sleep, and academic achievement: evidence of harm due to caffeine. , 2011, Journal of adolescence.

[17]  B. Mullan,et al.  A systematic review of the quality, content, and context of breakfast consumption , 2010 .

[18]  Martin E. P. Seligman,et al.  Positive education: positive psychology and classroom interventions , 2009 .

[19]  R. Singleton,et al.  Alcohol consumption, sleep, and academic performance among college students. , 2009, Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs.

[20]  H. Praag Exercise and the brain: something to chew on , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  J. Jolles,et al.  Age dependence of plasma phospholipid fatty acid levels: potential role of linoleic acid in the age-associated increase in docosahexaenoic acid and eicosapentaenoic acid concentrations , 2009, British Journal of Nutrition.

[22]  T. Visscher,et al.  Skipping breakfast, alcohol consumption and physical inactivity as risk factors for overweight and obesity in adolescents: results of the E-MOVO project , 2009, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[23]  H. van Praag,et al.  Exercise and the brain: something to chew on. , 2009, Trends in neurosciences.

[24]  E. De bruin,et al.  The combined effects of L-theanine and caffeine on cognitive performance and mood , 2008, Nutritional neuroscience.

[25]  F. Gomez-Pinilla,et al.  Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  C. Ruxton The impact of caffeine on mood, cognitive function, performance and hydration: a review of benefits and risks , 2008 .

[27]  P. Rogers Caffeine, mood and mental performance in everyday life , 2007 .

[28]  R. Mckeown,et al.  Cognitive performance is associated with macronutrient intake in healthy young and middle-aged adults , 2006, Nutritional neuroscience.

[29]  Andrew P. Smith,et al.  Effects of caffeine in overnight-withdrawn consumers and non-consumers , 2006, Nutritional neuroscience.

[30]  P. Rogers,et al.  Effects of caffeine on performance and mood: withdrawal reversal is the most plausible explanation , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[31]  J Viikari,et al.  Consistent dietary patterns identified from childhood to adulthood: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study , 2005, British Journal of Nutrition.

[32]  G. Rampersaud,et al.  Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic performance in children and adolescents. , 2005, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[33]  B. Ames,et al.  Is docosahexaenoic acid, an n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid, required for development of normal brain function? An overview of evidence from cognitive and behavioral tests in humans and animals. , 2005, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[34]  C. Muller,et al.  Body Mass Index, Academic Achievement, and School Context: Examining the Educational Experiences of Adolescents at Risk of Obesity∗ , 2004, Journal of health and social behavior.

[35]  P. Rogers,et al.  Mood and Cognitive Performance Effects of "Energy" Drink Constituents: Caffeine, Glucose and Carbonation , 2004, Nutritional neuroscience.

[36]  G. Block,et al.  The Effect of Breakfast Type on Total Daily Energy Intake and Body Mass Index: Results from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III) , 2003, Journal of the American College of Nutrition.

[37]  A. Rissanen,et al.  Breakfast skipping and health-compromising behaviors in adolescents and adults , 2003, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.

[38]  R. Magill,et al.  Effects of Tyrosine, Phentermine, Caffeine d-amphetamine, and Placebo on Cognitive and Motor Performance Deficits During Sleep Deprivation , 2003, Nutritional neuroscience.

[39]  O. Jegede,et al.  Gender Differences in Factors Influencing Achievement of Distance Education Students , 2001 .

[40]  J. Jolles,et al.  Habitual caffeine consumption and its relation to memory, attention, planning capacity and psychomotor performance across multiple age groups , 2000, Human psychopharmacology.

[41]  Cheryl L Grady,et al.  Changes in memory processing with age , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  E. Deci,et al.  Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[43]  E. Pollitt,et al.  Breakfast and cognition: an integrative summary. , 1998, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[44]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Achievement Motivation Goals in Relation to Academic Performance in Traditional and Nontraditional College Students , 1997 .

[45]  D. Halpern,et al.  Sex differences in intelligence. Implications for education. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[46]  Frank M. Bernt,et al.  Study practices and attitudes related to academic success in a distance learning programme , 1993 .

[47]  Astrid Nehlig,et al.  Caffeine and the central nervous system: mechanisms of action, biochemical, metabolic and psychostimulant effects , 1992, Brain Research Reviews.

[48]  R. Larsen,et al.  The Satisfaction with Life Scale , 1985, Journal of personality assessment.