Attentional factors in response time variability after traumatic brain injury: An ERP study

Reaction time (RT) is often used in the assessment of patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI), presumably because it reflects either information processing speed or attentional capacity. To clarify this distinction, we examined behavioral RT and the within-subject variability of RT as they relate to electrophysiological measures of attention and information processing. These include the P300 latency, which reflects stimulus evaluation time, P300 amplitude, which reflects attentional allocation, and the preresponse component of the contingent negative variation (CNV), which reflects sustained attention. We found that the latency and variability in behavioral RT were not correlated with the latency or variability of the P300, suggesting that stimulus evaluation time is not a major contributor to RT and its variability in this paradigm. However, among normal controls, RT was related to P300 amplitude, and therefore to attentional allocation. For the TBI subjects, it was the variability, not the speed, of RT that was related to P300 amplitude and to the preresponse component of the CNV. These data suggest that, while in normal controls RT reflects attentional allocation, among TBI subjects it is the variability in RT that is sensitive to the ability to allocate and sustain attention.

[1]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Terminal CNV in the absence of motor response. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[2]  J. Tecce Contingent negative variation (CNV) and psychological processes in man. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  津田 敏雄,et al.  Contingent negative variation , 1984 .

[4]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Speed of information processing, health, and cognitive performance in older adults , 1992 .

[5]  Sidney J. Segalowitz,et al.  Attentional allocation and capacity in waking arousal. , 1994 .

[6]  E. Donchin,et al.  On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[7]  D. Gronwall,et al.  The psychological effects of concussion , 1974 .

[8]  A. Papanicolaou,et al.  Slow magnetic flux from human frontal cortex. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Sources of P300 attenuation after head injury: single-trial amplitude, latency jitter, and EEG power. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[10]  J J Tecce,et al.  Contingent negative variation and the distraction--arousal hypothesis. , 1976, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[11]  W. A. Cobb,et al.  The Responsive Brain , 1977 .

[12]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .

[13]  R. Knight,et al.  Contributions of temporal-parietal junction to the human auditory P3 , 1989, Brain Research.

[14]  L. Kaufman,et al.  The hippocampal formation as a source of the slow endogenous potentials. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[15]  T. Tsubokawa,et al.  Motivational slow negative potential shift (CNV) related to thalamotomy. , 1978, Applied neurophysiology.

[16]  E. Halgren,et al.  The intracranial topography of the P3 event-related potential elicited during auditory oddball. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  The reliability of ERP components in the auditory oddball paradigm. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[18]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Cleverness and wisdom in 12‐year‐olds: Electrophysiological evidence for late maturation of the frontal lobe , 1992 .

[19]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  CNV evidence for the distinctiveness of frontal and posterior neural processes in a traumatic brain-injured population. , 1992, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[20]  E Donchin,et al.  A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.

[21]  S P Wise,et al.  A neurophysiological comparison of three distinct regions of the primate frontal lobe. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  J. Polich,et al.  Attention, probability, and task demands as determinants of P300 latency from auditory stimuli. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[23]  E. Miller,et al.  Simple and choice reaction time following severe head injury. , 1970, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[24]  R. Johnson,et al.  On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[25]  K. Campbell,et al.  Electrophysiological assessment of cognitive disorder in closed head-injured outpatients. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[26]  McKay Moore Sohlberg,et al.  Introduction to cognitive rehabilitation : theory and practice , 1989 .

[27]  C D Wickens,et al.  The dynamics of P300 during dual-task performance. , 1980, Progress in brain research.

[28]  W. Ritter,et al.  Orienting and habituation to auditory stimuli: a study of short term changes in average evoked responses. , 1968, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[30]  L. Squire,et al.  P3-like brain waves in normal monkeys and in monkeys with medial temporal lesions. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[31]  E. Donchin,et al.  On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[32]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  Definition, Identification, and Reliability of Measurement of the P300 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential , 1987 .

[33]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Characterization of stability of performance in patients with traumatic brain injury: Variability and consistency on reaction time tests. , 1994 .

[34]  D. Stuss,et al.  Reaction time after head injury: fatigue, divided and focused attention, and consistency of performance. , 1989, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[35]  S. Rosen,et al.  Steady potential shifts and facilitated learning of delayed response in monkeys , 1977, Experimental Neurology.

[36]  G. Geffen,et al.  Covert orientation of visual attention after closed head injury , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  A W Gaillard,et al.  Task and driving performance of patients with a severe concussion of the brain. , 1986, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[38]  J Wei,et al.  ELIMINATION OF CONDITIONING MOVEMENT COMPONENT IN WAVE EML AND DIFFERENCE BETWEEN WAVES V AND C , 1990 .

[39]  T. Salthouse A Theory of Cognitive Aging , 1985 .

[40]  H. Bauer,et al.  Concept identification as a function of preceding negative or positive spontaneous shifts in slow brain potentials. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[41]  W. Walter,et al.  Contingent Negative Variation : An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain , 1964, Nature.

[42]  Sidney J. Segalowitz,et al.  Skilled performance, practice, and the differentiation of speed-up from automatization effects: Evidence from second language word recognition , 1993, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[43]  R. Kail,et al.  Processing speed, speech rate, and memory. , 1992 .

[44]  A W Gaillard,et al.  The late CNV wave: preparation versus expectancy. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[45]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[46]  H. J. Eysenck,et al.  Speed of information processing, reaction time, and the theory of intelligence. , 1987 .

[47]  S. Foote,et al.  Long-latency event-related potentials in squirrel monkeys: further characterization of wave form morphology, topography, and functional properties. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[48]  M. Humphreys,et al.  Effects of closed-head injury on attentional processes: an information-processing stage analysis. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[49]  G. F. Tremblay,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex , 1989, Neurology.

[50]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[51]  R Parasuraman,et al.  Detection and recognition: Concurrent processes in perception , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.