The human motor system alters its reaching movement plan for task-irrelevant, positional forces.

The minimum intervention principle and the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis state that our nervous system only responds to force perturbations and sensorimotor noise if they affect task success. This idea has been tested in muscle and joint coordinate frames and more recently using workspace redundancy (e.g., reaching to large targets). However, reaching studies typically involve spatial and or temporal constraints. Constrained reaches represent a small proportion of movements we perform daily and may limit the emergence of natural behavior. Using more relaxed constraints, we conducted two reaching experiments to test the hypothesis that humans respond to task-relevant forces and ignore task-irrelevant forces. We found that participants responded to both task-relevant and -irrelevant forces. Interestingly, participants experiencing a task-irrelevant force, which simply pushed them into a different area of a large target and had no bearing on task success, changed their movement trajectory prior to being perturbed. These movement trajectory changes did not counteract the task-irrelevant perturbations, as shown in previous research, but rather were made into new areas of the workspace. A possible explanation for this behavior change is that participants were engaging in active exploration. Our data have implications for current models and theories on the control of biological motion.

[1]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[2]  References , 1971 .

[3]  P. Taylor,et al.  Test of optimal sampling by foraging great tits , 1978 .

[4]  N. Hogan Adaptive control of mechanical impedance by coactivation of antagonist muscles , 1984 .

[5]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  Sebastian Thrun,et al.  Efficient Exploration In Reinforcement Learning , 1992 .

[7]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[9]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Smoothness maximization along a predefined path accurately predicts the speed profiles of complex arm movements. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  J. Biesmeijer,et al.  Exploration and exploitation of food sources by social insect colonies: a revision of the scout-recruit concept , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[12]  D. Domkin,et al.  Structure of joint variability in bimanual pointing tasks , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Kelvin E. Jones,et al.  Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Joint angle variability in 3D bimanual pointing: uncontrolled manifold analysis , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  J. Scholz,et al.  Learning a throwing task is associated with differential changes in the use of motor abundance , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[19]  Jesse Hoey,et al.  An analytic solution to discrete Bayesian reinforcement learning , 2006, ICML.

[20]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  The role of kinematic redundancy in adaptation of reaching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Marc Mangel,et al.  Exploration or exploitation: life expectancy changes the value of learning in foraging strategies , 2007 .

[22]  Rieko Osu,et al.  Endpoint Stiffness of the Arm Is Directionally Tuned to Instability in the Environment , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Evidence for the Flexible Sensorimotor Strategies Predicted by Optimal Feedback Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Toward a new theory of motor synergies. , 2007, Motor control.

[25]  I. Iheanacho Exploration or exploitation? , 2007, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[26]  A. Faisal,et al.  Noise in the nervous system , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  P. Filzmoser,et al.  Statistical Data Analysis Explained , 2008 .

[28]  Clemens Reimann,et al.  Statistical data analysis explained : applied environmental statics with R , 2008 .

[29]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Structured variability of muscle activations supports the minimal intervention principle of motor control. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Olivier White,et al.  Use-Dependent and Error-Based Learning of Motor Behaviors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning from Sensory and Reward Prediction Errors during Motor Adaptation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[32]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy) , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Robert J. van Beers,et al.  How Does Our Motor System Determine Its Learning Rate? , 2012, PloS one.

[34]  Robert J. van Beers,et al.  How does our motor system determine its learning rate , 2012 .

[35]  Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al.  Beside the point: motor adaptation without feedback-based error correction in task-irrelevant conditions. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Stephen H Scott,et al.  Influence of the behavioral goal and environmental obstacles on rapid feedback responses. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Ramesh Balasubramaniam,et al.  Multijoint error compensation mediates unstable object control. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Aymar de Rugy,et al.  Muscle Coordination Is Habitual Rather than Optimal , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Random walk of motor planning in task-irrelevant dimensions. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry Trial-to-trial reoptimization of motor behavior due to changes in task demands is limited. , 2013 .

[41]  J. O. de Xivry Trial-to-Trial Reoptimization of Motor Behavior Due to Changes in Task Demands Is Limited , 2013, PLoS ONE.

[42]  Yohsuke R. Miyamoto,et al.  Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  Frédéric Crevecoeur,et al.  Rapid Online Selection between Multiple Motor Plans , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.