Ten-month-old infants infer the value of goals from the costs of actions

Ranking valuations on the basis of observed choices Obliged to make a choice between two goals, we evaluate the benefits of achieving the goals compared with the costs of the actions required before deciding what to do. This seems perfectly straightforward, and it is unsurprising to learn that we can also apply this reasoning to others; that is, someone that we see choosing a goal that requires a more costly action must value that goal more highly. What is remarkable, as Liu et al. report, is that preverbal children can reason in this same fashion. Science, this issue p. 1038 Infants can assess how worthwhile or valuable an object or goal may be from others’ behaviors in achieving or acquiring it. Infants understand that people pursue goals, but how do they learn which goals people prefer? We tested whether infants solve this problem by inverting a mental model of action planning, trading off the costs of acting against the rewards actions bring. After seeing an agent attain two goals equally often at varying costs, infants expected the agent to prefer the goal it attained through costlier actions. These expectations held across three experiments that conveyed cost through different physical path features (height, width, and incline angle), suggesting that an abstract variable—such as “force,” “work,” or “effort”—supported infants’ inferences. We modeled infants’ expectations as Bayesian inferences over utility-theoretic calculations, providing a bridge to recent quantitative accounts of action understanding in older children and adults.

[1]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2007, Developmental science.

[2]  J. Bentham An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation , 1945, Princeton Readings in Political Thought.

[3]  Charles Kemp,et al.  How to Grow a Mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction , 2011, Science.

[4]  S. Carey,et al.  Infants’ causal representations of state change events , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  E. Spelke,et al.  Preverbal infants identify emotional reactions that are incongruent with goal outcomes , 2014, Cognition.

[6]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Toward a Rational and Mechanistic Account of Mental Effort. , 2017, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  Can infants make transitive inferences? , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  Intentions L. Woodward Infants' Grasp of Others' , 2009 .

[9]  S. Carey,et al.  First-person action experience reveals sensitivity to action efficiency in prereaching infants , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Church: a language for generative models , 2008, UAI.

[11]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[12]  György Gergely,et al.  One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[13]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Mind Games: Game Engines as an Architecture for Intuitive Physics , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  H. Laborit,et al.  [Experimental study]. , 1958, Bulletin mensuel - Societe de medecine militaire francaise.

[15]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[16]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Can a Self-Propelled Box Have a Goal? , 2005, Psychological science.

[17]  Z. Nadasdy,et al.  Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.

[18]  G. Csibra,et al.  Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naı̈ve theory of rational action , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Ben Pelzer,et al.  influence.ME: Tools for Detecting Influential Data in Mixed Effects Models , 2012, R J..

[20]  H. Wellman,et al.  Young Children Use Statistical Sampling to Infer the Preferences of Other People , 2010, Psychological science.

[21]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Psychological Reasoning in Infancy. , 2016, Annual review of psychology.

[22]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Children’s understanding of the costs and rewards underlying rational action , 2015, Cognition.

[23]  T. Rai,et al.  Cultural Evolution , 2018, Encyclopedic Dictionary of Archaeology.

[24]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[25]  Chris L. Baker,et al.  Action understanding as inverse planning , 2009, Cognition.

[26]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[27]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[28]  D. Dennett The Intentional Stance. , 1987 .

[29]  G. Csibra,et al.  Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.

[30]  Máté Lengyel,et al.  Statistical Treatment of Looking-Time Data , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[31]  Charles Kemp,et al.  A decision network account of reasoning about other people’s choices , 2015, Cognition.

[32]  D. Bernoulli Exposition of a New Theory on the Measurement of Risk , 1954 .

[33]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[34]  Markus Paulus Action mirroring and action understanding: an ideomotor and attentional account , 2012, Psychological research.

[35]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  The Naïve Utility Calculus: Computational Principles Underlying Commonsense Psychology , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[37]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  The Child as Econometrician: A Rational Model of Preference Understanding in Children , 2014, PloS one.

[38]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  Recognizing the role of perception in action at 6 months. , 2009, Developmental science.

[39]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Five-month-old infants know humans are solid, like inanimate objects , 2006, Cognition.

[40]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: experiments in preverbal infants and a computational model. , 2013, Developmental science.

[41]  A. Woodward Infants' Grasp of Others' Intentions , 2009, Current directions in psychological science.

[42]  C. Heyes,et al.  The cultural evolution of mind reading , 2014, Science.

[43]  Edward Vul,et al.  Pure Reasoning in 12-Month-Old Infants as Probabilistic Inference , 2011, Science.

[44]  Peter Norvig,et al.  Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach , 1995 .

[45]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[46]  E S Spelke,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to effects of gravity on visible object motion. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Six-month-old infants expect agents to minimize the cost of their actions , 2017, Cognition.

[48]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs? , 2005, Science.

[49]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? , 2007, Cognition.

[50]  Wouter Kool,et al.  Cost-Benefit Arbitration Between Multiple Reinforcement-Learning Systems , 2017, Psychological science.