Common Neural Mechanisms for Response Selection and Perceptual Processing

Behavioral evidence supports a dissociation between response selection (RS; stimulus-to-response [SR] mapping) and perceptual discrimination (PD): The former may be subject to a central processing bottleneck, whereas the latter is not (Pashler, 1994). We previously (Jiang & Kanwisher, 2003) identified a set of frontal and parietal regions involved in RS as those that produce a stronger signal when subjects follow a difficult SR mapping rule than an easy mapping rule. Here, we test whether any of these regions are selectively activated by RS and not perceptual processing, as predicted by the central bottleneck view. In Experiment 1, subjects indicated which of four parallel lines was unique in length; PD was indexed by a higher BOLD response when the discrimination was difficult versus easy. Stimuli and responses were closely matched across conditions. We found that all regions-of-interest (ROIs) engaged by RS were also engaged by perceptual processing, arguing against the existence of mechanisms exclusively involved in RS. In Experiments 2 and 3, we asked what processes might go on in these ROIs, such that they could be recruited by both RS and perceptual processing. Our data argue against an account of this common activation in terms of spatial processing or general task difficulty. Thus, PD may recruit the same central processes that are engaged by RS.

[1]  H Pashler,et al.  Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[3]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[4]  H. Pashler Dissociations and dependencies between speed and accuracy: Evidence for a two-component theory of divided attention in simple tasks , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  W. Singer Synchronization of cortical activity and its putative role in information processing and learning. , 1993, Annual review of physiology.

[8]  A. Allport Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .

[9]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Attention and performance XIV (silver jubilee volume): synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuroscience , 1993 .

[10]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  H. Pashler,et al.  Visual fields Sequential operation of disconnected cerebral hemispheres in split-brain patients , 2002 .

[12]  R. Rafal Neglect , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[13]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Working Memory: A View from Neuroimaging , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  H. Pashler The Psychology of Attention , 1997 .

[15]  J. Desmond,et al.  Lobular Patterns of Cerebellar Activation in Verbal Working-Memory and Finger-Tapping Tasks as Revealed by Functional MRI , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[17]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  David E. Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .

[19]  D. Simons,et al.  Failure to detect changes to attended objects in motion pictures , 1997 .

[20]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. , 1998 .

[22]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  I. Rock,et al.  Inattentional blindness: Perception without attention. , 1998 .

[24]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Role of Parietal Cortex in Verbal Working Memory , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[26]  J. Desmond,et al.  Functional Specialization for Semantic and Phonological Processing in the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, NeuroImage.

[27]  M. Botvinick,et al.  The Contribution of the Anterior Cingulate Cortex to Executive Processes in Cognition , 1999, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[28]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[29]  E Ruthruff,et al.  Can practice eliminate the psychological refractory period effect? , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Neuroanatomic Overlap of Working Memory and Spatial Attention Networks: A Functional MRI Comparison within Subjects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[31]  Jonathan E. Jennings,et al.  An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue test reveals multiple color-selective areas in human ventral occipitotemporal cortex. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention , 1999, Neuron.

[33]  C R Genovese,et al.  Cortical networks subserving pursuit and saccadic eye movements in humans: An FMRI study , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[34]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  An event-related functional MRI study of the stroop color word interference task. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  A comparison of frontoparietal fMRI activation during anti-saccades and anti-pointing. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[37]  E. Crone,et al.  Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  R. Dell’Acqua,et al.  Visual encoding of patterns is subject to dual-task interference , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[40]  H. Pashler,et al.  The Psychology of Attention , 2000 .

[41]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Response Conflict : Effects of Response Modality and Processing Domain , 2022 .

[43]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  fMRI Studies of Stroop Tasks Reveal Unique Roles of Anterior and Posterior Brain Systems in Attentional Selection , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  J. Gore,et al.  Neural Correlates of the Attentional Blink , 2000, Neuron.

[45]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Neural Activation During Response Competition , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[47]  D. Schacter,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Executive Control: fMRI Evidence for Functional Distinctions within Lateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2001, NeuroImage.

[48]  C. Price,et al.  The Constraints Functional Neuroimaging Places on Classical Models of Auditory Word Processing , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  C. Frith,et al.  The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.

[50]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[51]  M. Petrides,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[53]  Jennifer M. Glass,et al.  Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.

[54]  J. Driver,et al.  Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction , 2001, Cognition.

[55]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[56]  J. Fiez,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fmri) Was Used to Investigate the Neural Substrates of Component Processes in Verbal Working Memory. Based on Behavioral Research Using , 2022 .

[57]  Scott A. Huettel,et al.  Dissociating the Neural Mechanisms of Visual Attention in Change Detection Using Functional MRI , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  H. Pashler,et al.  Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: Structural limitation or strategic postponement? , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[59]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. , 2001, Cerebral Cortex.

[60]  Stephen A. Engel,et al.  Neural Response to Perception of Volume in the Lateral Occipital Complex , 2001, Neuron.

[61]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[62]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[63]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[64]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus , 2002, Neuroreport.

[66]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[67]  J. Duncan,et al.  Separate and Shared Sources of Dual-Task Cost in Stimulus Identification and Response Selection , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[68]  T. Hendler,et al.  Object-completion effects in the human lateral occipital complex. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[69]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Neural implementation of response selection in humans as revealed by localized effects of stimulus–response compatibility on brain activation , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[70]  Eric H. Schumacher,et al.  Neural Mechanisms for Response Selection: Representation Specific or Modality Independent? , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Common Neural Substrates for Response Selection across Modalities and Mapping Paradigms , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[72]  Yuhong Jiang,et al.  Resolving dual-task interference: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[73]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Numerical Magnitude in the Human Parietal Lobe Tests of Representational Generality and Domain Specificity , 2004, Neuron.