Is Limb Proprioception a Function of the Limbs' Intertial Eigenvectors?
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Vincent Hayward,et al. Physical modeling applies to physiology, too , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[2] E. Bizzi,et al. Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[3] C Ghez,et al. Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.
[4] Michelle Fleury,et al. The mapping of visual space is a function of the structure of the visual field , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[5] M T Turvey,et al. Role of the inertia tensor in perceiving object orientation by dynamic touch. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] H. Solomon,et al. Movement-produced invariants in haptic explorations: An example of a self-organizing, information-driven, intentional system , 1988 .
[7] C. Bard,et al. Bimanual Interference in a Deafferented Patient and Control Subjects , 1994 .
[8] Robert L. Sainburg,et al. Contributions of vision and proprioception to accuracy in limb movements , 1994 .
[9] J. F. Soechting,et al. Does position sense at the elbow reflect a sense of elbow joint angle or one of limb orientation? , 1982, Brain Research.
[10] M T Turvey,et al. Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] J. F. Soechting,et al. Transformation from Head- to Shoulder-Centered Representation of Target Direction in Arm Movements , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] J. F. Soechting,et al. Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[13] L. Cohen. Contributions of tactile, musculo-tendinous and joint mechanisms to position sense in human shoulder. , 1958, Journal of neurophysiology.
[14] J Paillard,et al. A proprioceptive contribution to the spatial encoding of position cues for ballistic movements. , 1974, Brain research.
[15] M. Turvey,et al. Haptically perceiving size at a distance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[16] F. Varela,et al. Ways of coloring: Comparative color vision as a case study for cognitive science , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[17] M. Turvey,et al. Exteroception and exproprioception by dynamic touch are different functions of the inertia tensor , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[18] J. F. Soechting,et al. Psychophysical determination of coordinate representation of human arm orientation , 1984, Neuroscience.
[19] Christopher C. Pagano,et al. Spatial frames for motor control would be commensurate with spatial frames for vision and proprioception, but what about control of energy flows? , 1995, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[20] J F Soechting,et al. A coordinate system for the synthesis of visual and kinesthetic information , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[21] J F Soechting,et al. Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.
[22] M T Turvey,et al. Role of the inertia tensor in haptically perceiving where an object is grasped. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] M T Turvey,et al. Can shape be perceived by dynamic touch? , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] Christopher C. Pagano,et al. Perceiving by Dynamic Touch the Distances Reachable With Irregular Objects , 1993 .
[25] M T Turvey,et al. Eigenvectors of the inertia tensor and perceiving the orientation of a hand-held object by dynamic touch , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] G. E. Alexander,et al. For effective sensorimotor processing must there be explicit representations and reconciliation of differing frames of reference? , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[27] M T Turvey,et al. Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[28] Michael T. Turvey,et al. Effortful touch with minimal movement , 1992 .
[29] Christopher C. Pagano,et al. THE INERTIA TENSOR AS A BASIS FOR THE PERCEPTION OF LIMB ORIENTATION , 1995 .
[30] M. Turvey,et al. Temporal patterning in cascade juggling. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] H. M. Reynolds. The inertial properties of the body and its segments , 1978 .
[32] Y. Lamarre,et al. Rapid elbow flexion in the absence of proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback. , 1987, Human neurobiology.
[33] M. Turvey,et al. Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscular sense” , 1994, Neuroscience.
[34] Ma Conway,et al. Handbook of perception and cognition , 1996 .
[35] Jean-Luc Velay,et al. Elbow position sense in man: Contrasting results in matching and pointing , 1989 .
[36] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[37] Domina Eberle Spencer,et al. Theory of Holors , 1986 .
[38] J. F. Soechting,et al. Moving effortlessly in three dimensions: does Donders' law apply to arm movement? , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.