Different aberrations raise contrast thresholds for single-letter identification in line with their effect on cross-correlation-based confusability.

We previously showed that different types of aberration defocus, coma, and secondary astigmatism affect reading performance via different mechanisms. In this paper, we show the contrary result that, for identification of isolated letters, the effects of rendering different types of aberration can be described by a single cross-correlation-based metric. Aberrations reduce the effective resolution of an optical system, quantified by the high-frequency fall-off of the modulation transfer function. They additionally cause spatial-frequency-dependent phase and contrast changes, which have a size-dependent effect on letter forms. We used contrast threshold as our performance measure, instead of distance acuity, to separate the effects of form alterations from those of resolution limits. This measure is additionally appropriate in comparing single-letter-based performance to reading at a fixed distance. The relationship between a cross-correlation-based measure of letter confusability and performance was the same for all three types of aberration. For reading, we had found a different relationship for coma than for defocus and secondary astigmatism. We conclude that even when two tasks--letter identification and reading--use the same component stimulus set, the combination of multiple letters in a reading task produces functional differences between the effects of these aberrations that are not present for isolated letters.

[1]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  The visual filter mediating letter identification , 1994, Nature.

[2]  G. Heath,et al.  The influence of visual acuity on accommodative responses of the eye. , 1956, American journal of optometry and archives of American Academy of Optometry.

[3]  David Williams,et al.  Image Metrics for Predicting Subjective Image Quality , 2005, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[4]  Potential signal to accommodation from the Stiles–Crawford effect and ocular monochromatic aberrations , 2007, Journal of modern optics.

[5]  Hannah E Smithson,et al.  Not all aberrations are equal: reading impairment depends on aberration type and magnitude. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[6]  D R Williams,et al.  Supernormal vision and high-resolution retinal imaging through adaptive optics. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[7]  James R Tresilian,et al.  Improving vision: neural compensation for optical defocus , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Andrew B Watson,et al.  Modeling acuity for optotypes varying in complexity. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[9]  Edwin J Sarver,et al.  Are all aberrations equal? , 2002, Journal of refractive surgery.

[10]  Sowmya Ravikumar,et al.  Visual Impact of Zernike and Seidel Forms of Monochromatic Aberrations , 2010, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[11]  余翔,et al.  Effects of Different Zernike Terms on Optical Quality and Vision of Human Eyes , 2009 .

[12]  Michael N Jones,et al.  Case-sensitive letter and bigram frequency counts from large-scale English corpora , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[13]  R. Legras,et al.  Effect of monochromatic induced aberrations on visual performance measured by adaptive optics technology. , 2010, Journal of refractive surgery.

[14]  Junzhong Liang,et al.  Aberrations and retinal image quality of the normal human eye. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[15]  Geunyoung Yoon,et al.  Vision improvement by correcting higher-order aberrations with phase plates in normal eyes. , 2004, Journal of refractive surgery.

[16]  D. Pelli,et al.  The role of spatial frequency channels in letter identification , 2002, Vision Research.

[17]  M. Netto,et al.  Wavefront-guided ablation: evidence for efficacy compared to traditional ablation. , 2006, American journal of ophthalmology.

[18]  Junzhong Liang,et al.  Objective measurement of wave aberrations of the human eye with the use of a Hartmann-Shack wave-front sensor. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[19]  A. Bradley,et al.  Phase changes induced by optical aberrations degrade letter and face acuity. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[20]  David R Williams,et al.  Neural compensation for the eye's optical aberrations. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[21]  Susana Marcos,et al.  Adapting to blur produced by ocular high-order aberrations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[22]  Kazunori Miyata,et al.  Ocular higher-order aberrations and contrast sensitivity after conventional laser in situ keratomileusis. , 2004, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[23]  Gordon E Legge,et al.  Higher-contrast requirements for recognizing low-pass-filtered letters. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[25]  Edwin J Sarver,et al.  Visual Acuity as a Function of Zernike Mode and Level of Root Mean Square Error , 2003, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[26]  L. O. Harvey,et al.  Efficient estimation of sensory thresholds with ML-PEST. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[27]  Susana Marcos,et al.  Visual performance with real-life tasks under adaptive-optics ocular aberration correction. , 2009, Journal of vision.