Investigating route learning, metacognition, and beacon-based strategies using virtual environments

Abstract Introduction Dominant theories of wayfinding may have underestimated the ease with which adults can learn a route, even with minimal exposure to that route. Objectives In this paper we present three different experiments to provide a comprehensive picture of whether adults can learn complex routes consisting of a number of choicepoints, and if so, what strategies they might be employing to do so. Method and results We found that adults could learn a 15-junction route after only a single experience of the route (Experiment 1) but that they underestimated how good they would be at learning the 15-junction route (Experiment 2). In Experiment 3, we found that when learning a route made up of “T” shaped junctions, participants relied on a “beacon” strategy based on visual matching. Conclusions Collectively, these findings suggest that adults can learn complex routes, even with as many as 15 choicepoints, very quickly and without the need for repeated exposure. These findings have implications for theories of wayfinding and call into question the need for repeated exposure.

[1]  Christoph Hölscher,et al.  Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making , 2011, CogSci.

[2]  Steve Pettifer,et al.  Transfer of route learning from virtual to real environments. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[3]  R. Beni,et al.  Age-differences in environment route learning: The role of input and recall-test modalities in young and older adults , 2012 .

[4]  V. Liberman,et al.  Local and global judgments of confidence. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  Spatial Knowledge Acquisition by Children: Route Learning and Relational Distances , 1992 .

[6]  D. Head,et al.  Age effects on wayfinding and route learning skills , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  Kai Hamburger,et al.  Multimodal Integration of Spatial Information: The Influence of Object-Related Factors and Self-Reported Strategies , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[8]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Two strategies for learning a route in a driving simulator. , 1997 .

[9]  Mark Blades,et al.  Using virtual environments to investigate wayfinding in 8- to 12-year-olds and adults. , 2018, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  Mark Blades,et al.  How useful are landmarks when learning a route in a virtual environment? Evidence from typical development and Williams syndrome. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  Jan M. Wiener,et al.  Human place and response learning: navigation strategy selection, pupil size and gaze behavior , 2016, Psychological research.

[12]  Alycia M. Hund,et al.  The effects of sense of direction and training experience on wayfinding efficiency , 2009 .

[13]  Yoshiaki Takeuchi,et al.  Individual differences in wayfinding strategies , 2003 .

[14]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[15]  Harry Heft The role of environmental features in route-learning: Two exploratory studies of way-finding , 1979 .

[16]  Carryl L. Baldwin,et al.  Individual Differences in Route-Learning Strategy and Associated Working Memory Resources , 2009, Hum. Factors.

[17]  C D Heth,et al.  Place recognition and way finding by children and adults , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[18]  R. Hart,et al.  The Development of Spatial Cognition: A Review. , 1973 .

[19]  J. C. Malinowski,et al.  INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN PERFORMANCE ON A LARGE-SCALE, REAL-WORLD WAYFINDING TASK , 2001 .

[20]  David M. Mark,et al.  Cognitive and Linguistic Aspects of Geographic Space: New Perspectives on Geographic Information Research , 1991 .

[21]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[22]  Tommy Gärling,et al.  Memory for the spatial layout of the everyday physical environment: Factors affecting rate of acquisition , 1981 .

[23]  Laura A. Carlson,et al.  Getting Lost in Buildings , 2010 .

[24]  C. Lawton,et al.  Gender Differences in Wayfinding Strategies and Anxiety About Wayfinding: A Cross-Cultural Comparison , 2002 .

[25]  M. Blades,et al.  The Cognition of Geographic Space , 2002 .

[26]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  Place recognition and wayfinding: Making sense of space , 1992 .

[27]  Mark Blades,et al.  The development of wayfinding abilities in children: learning routes with and without landmarks , 2015 .

[28]  M. Elsner,et al.  Where's Wally: the influence of visual salience on referring expression generation , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[29]  Tom Troscianko,et al.  Regional effects of clutter on human target detection performance. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[30]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Neural Encoding of Objects Relevant for Navigation and Resting State Correlations with Navigational Ability , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Nigel Foreman,et al.  Research Paradigms and Methodologies for Investigating Children's Wayfinding , 2013 .

[32]  M. Heil,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition in younger and elderly adults: a study in a virtual environment. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[33]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Navigating Buildings in "Desk-Top" Virtual Environments: Experimental Investigations Using Extended Navigational Experience , 1997 .

[34]  Tad T. Brunyé,et al.  The Path more Travelled: Time Pressure Increases Reliance on Familiar Route-Based Strategies during Navigation , 2017, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  C. Donald Heth,et al.  Self-ratings of sense of direction and route reversal performance , 2002 .

[36]  Thomas Wolbers,et al.  Maladaptive Bias for Extrahippocampal Navigation Strategies in Aging Humans , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Alexander Klippel,et al.  The Role of Structure and Function in the Conceptualization of Directions , 2012 .

[38]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Use of Virtual Environments to Investigate Development of Spatial Behavior and Spatial Knowledge of School-Age Children , 2007, Psychological reports.

[39]  Mark Blades,et al.  How do individuals with Williams syndrome learn a route in a real-world environment? , 2010, Developmental science.

[40]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Neural representation of navigational relevance is rapidly induced and long lasting. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Using desktop virtual environments to investigate the role of landmarks , 2002, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[42]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Memory for object location and route direction in virtual large-scale space , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  Andrew Tolmie,et al.  Egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies in Williams syndrome and typical development. , 2014, Developmental science.

[44]  D. Kimura,et al.  Sex differences in route-learning , 1993 .

[45]  L. Kozlowski,et al.  Sense of Direction, Spatial Orientation, and Cognitive Maps. , 1977 .

[46]  E. H. Cornell,et al.  Children's wayfinding: Response to instructions to use environmental landmarks. , 1989 .

[47]  P. Jansen-Osmann,et al.  The representation of landmarks and routes in children and adults: A study in a virtual environment , 2004 .

[48]  Danielle E. Matthews,et al.  Encouraging 5-year olds to attend to landmarks: a way to improve children's wayfinding strategies in a virtual environment , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[49]  Lin Wang,et al.  Development of Landmark Knowledge at Decision Points , 2014, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[50]  Toru Ishikawa,et al.  Retention of memory for large-scale spaces , 2013, Memory.

[51]  Takefumi Hayashi,et al.  Modeling the cognitive map formation process based on psychological experiments using a computer graphics system , 1990, 1990 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics Conference Proceedings.

[52]  Braj Bhushan,et al.  Effect of Landmark Type on Route Memory in Unfamiliar Homogenous Environment , 2017 .

[53]  A. Siegel,et al.  Way finding and cognitive mapping in large-scale environments: a test of a developmental model. , 1983, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[54]  M. Blades,et al.  Route knowledge and configural knowledge in typical and atypical development: a comparison of sparse and rich environments , 2015, Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders.

[55]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[56]  C. Lawton,et al.  Gender, Spatial Abilities, and Wayfinding , 2010 .

[57]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the environment: Individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of separately learned places , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  Mark Blades,et al.  The development of route learning in Down syndrome, Williams syndrome and typical development: investigations with virtual environments. , 2015, Developmental science.

[59]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies. , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[60]  Christopher A. Stevens,et al.  Are you sure the library is that way? Metacognitive monitoring of spatial judgments. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[61]  Y. Lippa,et al.  Landmarks as beacons and associative cues: Their role in route learning , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[62]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[63]  Arzu Çöltekin,et al.  Toward optimizing the design of virtual environments for route learning: empirically assessing the effects of changing levels of realism on memory , 2019, Int. J. Digit. Earth.

[64]  C. Lawton Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety , 1994 .

[65]  E. Coluccia,et al.  Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review , 2004 .

[66]  How Do Young Children Learn Novel Routes? The Importance of Landmarks in the Child's Retracing of Routes through the Large-Scale Environment. , 1984 .

[67]  Bettina Berendt,et al.  What makes a route appear longer? An experimental perspective on features, route segmentation, and distance knowledge , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[68]  E. H. Cornell,et al.  Adventure as a stimulus for cognitive development , 2001 .

[69]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Sex differences in spatial ability and spatial activities , 1983 .

[70]  R. Nori,et al.  Individual Differences in Visuo-Spatial Working Memory and Real-World Wayfinding , 2009 .

[71]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[72]  C. Collin,et al.  Gender differences in metacognitive judgments and performance on a goal-directed wayfinding task , 2019, Journal of Cognitive Psychology.

[73]  D. R. Montello A New Framework for Understanding the Acquisition of Spatial Knowledge in Large-Scale Environments , 1998 .

[74]  Bettina Berendt,et al.  Investigating Distance Knowledge Using Virtual Environments , 2002 .