Response time as a proxy of ongoing mental state: A combined fMRI and pupillometry study in Generalized Anxiety Disorder

ABSTRACT In Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD), fluctuations in ongoing thoughts (i.e., mind‐wandering) often take the form of rigid and intrusive perseverative cognition, such as worry. Here, we sought to characterise the neural correlates of mind‐wandering and perseverative cognition, alongside autonomic nervous system indices of central arousal, notably pupil dilation. We implemented a protocol incorporating the dynamic delivery of thought‐probes within a functional neuroimaging task. Sixteen individuals with GAD and sixteen matched healthy controls (HC) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging with concomitant pupillometry. Participants performed a series of low‐demand tracking tasks, responding to occasional changes in a target stimulus. Such a task is typically accompanied by self‐generated, off‐task thinking. Thought‐probes were triggered based on an individual's response time (RT) when responding to the change in the target. Subjective reports showed that long RT predicted off‐task thinking/mind‐wandering. Moreover, long RT and mind‐wandering were also associated with larger pupil diameter. This effect was exaggerated in GAD patients during perseverative cognition. Within brain, during both pre‐target periods and target events, there were distinct neural correlates for mind‐wandering (e.g., anterior cingulate and paracingulate activation at target onset) and perseverative cognition (e.g., opposite patterns of activation in posterior cingulate and cerebellum at target onset in HC and GAD). Results suggest that not only attention systems but also sensory‐motor cortices are important during off‐task states. Interestingly, changes across the ‘default mode network’ also tracked fluctuations in pupillary size. Autonomic expression in pupillary changes mirrors brain activation patterns that occur during different forms of repetitive thinking. HIGHLIGHTSThought‐probes were incorporated within a fMRI task with concomitant pupillometry.Reponse time (RT) predicted episodes of self‐generated, off‐task thinking.Long RT was associated with larger pupil diameter, especially in GAD during worry.Distinct neural correlates emerged for mind‐wandering and worry in GAD.Changes across the default mode network tracked fluctuations in pupillary size.

[1]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The restless mind. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  E. Klinger,et al.  Modes of Normal Conscious Flow , 1978 .

[3]  S. Faraone,et al.  Meta-Analysis of Structural Imaging Findings in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[4]  J. Beatty,et al.  The pupillary system. , 2000 .

[5]  Emily L. Dennis,et al.  Neural correlates of rumination in depression , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  B. Löwe,et al.  A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. , 2006, Archives of internal medicine.

[7]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  Neurobiological substrates of cognitive rigidity and autonomic inflexibility in generalized anxiety disorder , 2016, Biological Psychology.

[8]  A. Beck,et al.  Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation , 1988 .

[9]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  B. Penninx,et al.  Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety: A conceptual replication. , 2014, Behaviour research and therapy.

[11]  J. Smallwood,et al.  Segmenting the stream of consciousness: The psychological correlates of temporal structures in the time series data of a continuous performance task , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[12]  D. Margulies,et al.  Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Editorial: Can't Get You Out of My Head: Brain-Body Interactions in Perseverative Cognition , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Haakon G. Engen,et al.  Shaped by the Past: The Default Mode Network Supports Cognition that Is Independent of Immediate Perceptual Input , 2015, PloS one.

[15]  Zachary C. Irving,et al.  Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[16]  D. Sharp,et al.  The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  Joshua M. Carlson,et al.  VENTROMEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTEX REACTIVITY IS ALTERED IN GENERALIZED ANXIETY DISORDER DURING FEAR GENERALIZATION , 2013, Depression and anxiety.

[18]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[19]  J. Taylor,et al.  A personality scale of manifest anxiety. , 1953, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[20]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  On the relation of mind wandering and ADHD symptomatology , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[21]  R. O’Connell,et al.  Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[22]  Claire Braboszcz,et al.  Oculometric variations during mind wandering , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[23]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Tracking arousal state and mind wandering with pupillometry , 2018, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[24]  David K. Menon,et al.  Default mode contributions to automated information processing , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Evelyne Balteau,et al.  The Locus Ceruleus Is Involved in the Successful Retrieval of Emotional Memories in Humans , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Bradley,et al.  The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[27]  J. Jonides,et al.  Depression, rumination and the default network. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[28]  B. Peterson,et al.  Cerebellar Morphology and the Effects of Stimulant Medications in Youths with Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder , 2014, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[29]  J. Smallwood,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Semantic Retrieval: TMS Reveals Distinct Contributions of Posterior Middle Temporal Gyrus and Angular Gyrus , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Anu Asnaani,et al.  Gender differences in anxiety disorders: prevalence, course of illness, comorbidity and burden of illness. , 2011, Journal of psychiatric research.

[31]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  Goal Directed Worry Rules Are Associated with Distinct Patterns of Amygdala Functional Connectivity and Vagal Modulation during Perseverative Cognition , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[32]  Janet B W Williams Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[33]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  The verbal nature of worry in generalized anxiety: Insights from the brain , 2017, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Classical and Bayesian Inference in Neuroimaging: Applications , 2002, NeuroImage.

[35]  Bodily arousal differentially impacts stimulus processing and memory: Norepinephrine in interoception , 2016, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Julian F Thayer,et al.  The perseverative cognition hypothesis: a review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. , 2006, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[37]  J. Andrews-Hanna,et al.  Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[38]  Michael J Kane,et al.  Drifting from slow to "D'oh!": working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[39]  C. Beevers,et al.  Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Aaron Kucyi,et al.  Disrupted functional connectivity of cerebellar default network areas in attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[41]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  Alterations in Amygdala-Prefrontal Functional Connectivity Account for Excessive Worry and Autonomic Dysregulation in Generalized Anxiety Disorder , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.

[42]  Cristina Ottaviani,et al.  Flexibility as the key for somatic health: From mind wandering to perseverative cognition , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[43]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  Dimensions of Experience: Exploring the Heterogeneity of the Wandering Mind , 2018, Psychological science.

[44]  F. Ferri,et al.  Neural correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: a functional MRI study , 2009, Psychological Medicine.

[45]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Pupillary correlates of lapses of sustained attention , 2016, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  A. Gerlach,et al.  Superior perception of phasic physiological arousal and the detrimental consequences of the conviction to be aroused on worrying and metacognitions in GAD. , 2008, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[47]  Christian Sander,et al.  Evoked potentials and behavioral performance during different states of brain arousal , 2017, BMC Neuroscience.

[48]  V. Romei,et al.  Multisensory signalling enhances pupil dilation , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[49]  Cristina Ottaviani,et al.  Brain-heart interaction in perseverative cognition. , 2018, Psychophysiology.

[50]  A. M. Ruscio,et al.  Experience and appraisal of worry among high worriers with and without generalized anxiety disorder. , 2004, Behaviour research and therapy.

[51]  Jason S. McCarley,et al.  Mind Wandering Behind the Wheel , 2011, Hum. Factors.

[52]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Pupillometric Evidence for the Decoupling of Attention from Perceptual Input during Offline Thought , 2011, PloS one.

[53]  H. Critchley,et al.  Amygdala functional connectivity as a longitudinal biomarker of symptom changes in generalized anxiety , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[54]  M. Kane,et al.  Conducting the train of thought: working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[55]  M. Schlund,et al.  Effects of citalopram on worry and brain activation in patients with generalized anxiety disorder , 2004, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[56]  Mickaël Causse,et al.  Frequency analysis of a task-evoked pupillary response: Luminance-independent measure of mental effort. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[57]  W. Ray,et al.  Chronic worry and the temporal dynamics of emotional processing. , 2011, Emotion.

[58]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  Representing Representation: Integration between the Temporal Lobe and the Posterior Cingulate Influences the Content and Form of Spontaneous Thought , 2016, PloS one.

[59]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[60]  D. McDougal,et al.  Autonomic control of the eye. , 2015, Comprehensive Physiology.

[61]  C. Spielberger State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory , 2010 .

[62]  H. Critchley,et al.  Network abnormalities in generalized anxiety pervade beyond the amygdala-pre-frontal cortex circuit: Insights from graph theory , 2018, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[63]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[64]  Mickaël Causse,et al.  Neural and psychophysiological correlates of human performance under stress and high mental workload , 2016, Biological Psychology.

[65]  M. Bradley,et al.  Modulation of the initial light reflex during affective picture viewing. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[66]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Catching the mind in flight: Using behavioral indices to detect mindless reading in real time , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[67]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Fluctuating Minds: Spontaneous Psychophysical Variability during Mind-Wandering , 2016, PloS one.

[68]  Y. Weinstein Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with probes? Let me count the ways , 2018, Behavior research methods.

[69]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Purging of Memories from Conscious Awareness Tracked in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Window to the Wandering Mind: Pupillometry of Spontaneous Thought While Reading Window to the Wandering Mind: Pupillometry of Spontaneous Thought While Reading Pupillometry and Mind-wandering 2 Introduction , 2022 .

[71]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  The ebb and flow of attention: Between-subject variation in intrinsic connectivity and cognition associated with the dynamics of ongoing experience , 2019, NeuroImage.

[72]  H. Critchley,et al.  Cortical morphometric predictors of autonomic dysfunction in generalized anxiety disorder , 2019, Autonomic Neuroscience.

[73]  J. Thayer,et al.  Physiological concomitants of perseverative cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis. , 2016, Psychological bulletin.

[74]  Almudena Duque,et al.  Gaze-fixation and pupil dilation in the processing of emotional faces: The role of rumination , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[75]  J. Thayer,et al.  Individual differences in resting heart rate variability moderate thought suppression success. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[76]  J. P. Hamilton,et al.  Depressive Rumination, the Default-Mode Network, and the Dark Matter of Clinical Neuroscience , 2015, Biological Psychiatry.

[77]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  When attention wanders: Pupillometric signatures of fluctuations in external attention , 2017, Cognition.

[78]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neurostructural abnormalities associated with axes of emotion dysregulation in generalized anxiety , 2015, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[79]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  T J Meyer,et al.  Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. , 1990, Behaviour research and therapy.

[81]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  Distant from input: Evidence of regions within the default mode network supporting perceptually-decoupled and conceptually-guided cognition , 2018, NeuroImage.

[82]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes , 2015, NeuroImage.

[83]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Do the Seconds Turn Into Hours? Relationships between Sustained Pupil Dilation in Response to Emotional Information and Self-Reported Rumination , 2003, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[84]  Christian Sander,et al.  Brain and Body Associations Between EEG-Vigilance and the Autonomous Nervous System Activity During Rest , 2011 .