Physiological Correlates of the Big 5: Autonomic Responses to Video Presentations

Abstract Personality’s link to emotional experience has been demonstrated, but specific biological responses to emotion as a function of personality have not been well-established. Here, the association between personality and physiological responses (heart rate, skin conductance, and respiration) to emotional videos was assessed. One-hundred sixty-nine participants self-reported on their Big 5 personality traits and underwent ambulatory monitoring as they watched four brief video clips from primetime television content showing scenes containing violence, fear, sadness, and tension. Generally, the negatively-toned emotional scenes provoked increases in skin conductance response and declines in heart rate. We found that physiological outcomes depended on the particular emotional scene and on personality, most notably Extraversion and Neuroticism. Extraversion, and to a lesser degree, Neuroticism, were associated with increases in autonomic arousal responses to the scenes. Gender also interacted with personality to predict responses, such that women who scored higher on measures of Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Conscientious tended to show more physiological arousal than men. Overall, the emotional scenes evoked increases in arousal and more controlled attention. The findings are discussed in context of the limited capacity model and shed light on how personality and gender affect physiological reactions to emotional experiences in everyday life.

[1]  R. Constable,et al.  Attentional bias for valenced stimuli as a function of personality in the dot-probe task , 2004 .

[2]  P. Costa,et al.  Adding Liebe und Arbeit: The Full Five-Factor Model and Well-Being , 1991 .

[3]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Principles of psychophysiology : physical, social, and inferential elements , 1990 .

[4]  J. Desmond,et al.  An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  D. Funder,et al.  Evaluating correlations in studies of personality and behavior: Beyond the number of significant findings to be expected by chance , 2009 .

[6]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[7]  Sylvia D. Kreibig,et al.  Cardiovascular, electrodermal, and respiratory response patterns to fear- and sadness-inducing films. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[8]  Judith A. Hall Nonverbal sex differences : communication accuracy and expressive style , 1984 .

[9]  C. Malatesta,et al.  The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[10]  M. Quirin,et al.  Gender Differences in Psychophysiological Responses to Disgust , 2008 .

[11]  M. Erb,et al.  Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: quality, quantity, time course and gender differences , 2001, Psychiatry Research.

[12]  M. Thase,et al.  Can’t shake that feeling: event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[13]  Marina Krcmar,et al.  Uses and Gratifications of Media Violence: Personality Correlates of Viewing and Liking Violent Genres , 2005 .

[14]  A. Yoshino,et al.  Relationships between temperament dimensions in personality and unconscious emotional responses , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Amygdala Response to Happy Faces as a Function of Extraversion , 2002, Science.

[16]  J. Suls,et al.  Cardiovascular reactivity and neuroticism: results from a laboratory and controlled ambulatory stress protocol. , 1999, Journal of personality.

[17]  J. Gross,et al.  Emotion-Regulation Choice , 2011, Psychological science.

[18]  A. Raine,et al.  Serious Delinquent Behavior, Sensation Seeking, and Electrodermal Arousal , 2002, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[19]  Michael W. Kraus,et al.  Class and compassion: socioeconomic factors predict responses to suffering. , 2012, Emotion.

[20]  Paul De Boeck,et al.  From BIS/BAS to the big five , 2006 .

[21]  P. Costa,et al.  Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: happy and unhappy people. , 1980, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Judith A. Hall Gender Effects in Decoding Nonverbal Cues , 1978 .

[23]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Do extraverts process social stimuli differently from introverts? , 2011, Cognitive neuroscience.

[24]  Annie Lang,et al.  The effects of production pacing and arousing content on the information processing of television messages , 1999 .

[25]  P. Ekman,et al.  Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[26]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Neuroticism is associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to emotionally evocative pictures. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[27]  D. Derryberry,et al.  Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal? , 2005, Emotion.

[28]  M. Zuckerman Psychobiology of Personality: Subject Index , 2005 .

[29]  B. Bryant The neighborhood walk: sources of support in middle childhood. , 1985, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[30]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The Affect System , 1999 .

[31]  E. McClure A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  H. Eysenck Biological Basis of Personality , 1963, Nature.

[33]  Annie Lang,et al.  It's an Arousing, Fast-Paced Kind of World: The Effects of Age and Sensation Seeking on the Information Processing of Substance-Abuse PSAs , 2005 .

[34]  Annie Lang,et al.  The limited capacity model of mediated message processing , 2000 .

[35]  D. Watson,et al.  On traits and temperament: general and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five-factor model. , 1992, Journal of personality.

[36]  R. Larsen,et al.  Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  John M. Zelenski,et al.  Susceptibility to affect: a comparison of three personality taxonomies. , 1999, Journal of personality.

[38]  R Todd Constable,et al.  A double dissociation between mood states and personality traits in the anterior cingulate. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.

[39]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Psychobiology of personality , 1991 .

[40]  R. McFatter Emotional intensity: Some components and their relations to extra version and neuroticism , 1998 .

[41]  Małgorzata Kossowska,et al.  Do it your own way: Cognitive strategies, intelligence, and personality , 1994 .

[42]  J. Gray,et al.  The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. , 1970, Behaviour research and therapy.

[43]  S. Gosling,et al.  A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains , 2003 .

[44]  P. Lang,et al.  Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. , 1989 .

[45]  P. Lang,et al.  Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[46]  S. Srivastava,et al.  The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. , 1999 .

[47]  G. Turpin,et al.  Effects of stimulus intensity on autonomic responding: the problem of differentiating orienting and defense reflexes. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[48]  E. Diener,et al.  Feeling bad? The “Power” of positive thinking may not apply to everyone , 2009 .

[49]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  May I have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli , 2003, Neuropsychologia.