Default processing of event sequences.

In a wide range of circumstances, it is important to perceive and represent the sequence of events. For example, sequence perception is necessary to learn statistical contingencies between events, and to generate predictions about events when segmenting actions. However, viewer's awareness of event sequence is rarely tested, and at least some means of encoding event sequence are likely to be resource-intensive. Therefore, previous research may have overestimated the degree to which viewers are aware of specific event sequences. In the experiments reported here, we tested viewers' ability to detect anomalies during visual event sequences. Participants viewed videos containing events that either did or did not contain an out-of-order action. Participants were unable to consistently detect the misordered events, and performance on the task decreased significantly to very low levels when performing a secondary task. In addition, participants almost never detected misorderings in an incidental version of the task, and performance increased when videos ended immediately after the misordering, We argue that these results demonstrate that viewers can effectively perceive the elements of events, but do not consistently test their expectations about the specific sequence of natural events unless bidden to do so by task-specific demands. (PsycINFO Database Record

[1]  Tinka Welke,et al.  Investigating dimensional organization in scripts using the pupillary response. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[2]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Generating predictive inferences while viewing a movie , 1996 .

[3]  A. Young,et al.  Configurational Information in Face Perception , 1987, Perception.

[4]  K. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Neuronal Processing Delays Are Compensated in the Sensorimotor Branch of the Visual System , 2003, Current Biology.

[5]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Human brain activity time-locked to perceptual event boundaries , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[7]  S. Schwan,et al.  The cognitive representation of filmic event summaries , 2004 .

[8]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  B. Breitmeyer,et al.  Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: A comparison, review, and update , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Ronald A. Rensink Visual Search for Change: A Probe into the Nature of Attentional Processing , 2000 .

[11]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event perception: a mind-brain perspective. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Predictive action in infancy: tracking and reaching for moving objects , 1998, Cognition.

[13]  M K Kaiser,et al.  How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. , 1995, Science.

[14]  J. Freyd,et al.  A velocity effect for representational momentum , 1985 .

[15]  M. Potter,et al.  Recognition memory for a rapid sequence of pictures. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  Ori Friedman,et al.  Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’ , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Two strategies for learning a route in a driving simulator. , 1997 .

[18]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  G. Loftus,et al.  Perceptual and conceptual masking of pictures. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  P. A. Kolers,et al.  Shape and color in apparent motion , 1976, Vision Research.

[21]  D. Ballard,et al.  What you see is what you need. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[22]  Darren Newtson,et al.  The perceptual organization of ongoing behavior , 1976 .

[23]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[24]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Segmenting dynamic human action via statistical structure , 2008, Cognition.

[25]  M. Potter Short-term conceptual memory for pictures. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[26]  D. Simons,et al.  Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction , 1998 .

[27]  T. Hubbard Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  J. Decety,et al.  Distinct Areas in Parietal Cortex Involved in Long-Term and Short-Term Action Planning: A Pet Investigation , 2002, Cortex.

[29]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. , 1994, Psychological review.

[30]  D. Ballard,et al.  Memory Representations in Natural Tasks , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Rachel Keen,et al.  Representation of Objects and Events , 2003 .

[32]  A. Endress,et al.  The Social Sense: Susceptibility to Others’ Beliefs in Human Infants and Adults , 2010, Science.

[33]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes , 1997 .

[34]  Norman G. Vinson,et al.  Sources of object-specific effects in representational momentum , 2002 .

[35]  T. Braver The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  M. Hayhoe Vision Using Routines: A Functional Account of Vision , 2000 .

[37]  Dale J. Barr,et al.  Mindreading in an exotic case: the normal adult human , 2005 .

[38]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Encoding of Sequence and Boundaries of Scripts Following Prefrontal Lesions , 1996, Cortex.

[39]  Tinka Welke,et al.  I spy with my little eye: detection of temporal violations in event sequences and the pupillary response. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[40]  Daniel T. Levin,et al.  The role of relational triggers in event perception , 2015, Cognition.

[41]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Prediction Error Associated with the Perceptual Segmentation of Naturalistic Events , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Kevin J. Riggs,et al.  Is Belief Reasoning Automatic? , 2006, Psychological science.

[43]  Ian M. Thornton,et al.  Anticipating action in complex scenes , 2004 .

[44]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  Daniel J. Simons,et al.  Current Approaches to Change Blindness , 2000 .

[46]  Nicholas D. Duran,et al.  Prediction during statistical learning, and implications for the implicit/explicit divide , 2012, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[47]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[48]  M. Sinico Benussi and the history of temporal displacement , 1999 .

[49]  Brent Strickland,et al.  Event completion: Event based inferences distort memory in a matter of seconds , 2011, Cognition.

[50]  Stephanie Kelter,et al.  Comprehending narratives containing flashbacks: evidence for temporally organized representations. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[51]  Stephan Schwan,et al.  The Role of Segmentation in Perception and Understanding of Events , 2008 .

[52]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.