Sentence processing in children with early unilateral brain injury

This study examined how children use word order and animacy cues to determine the agent of the action in an on-line sentence-comprehension task. The subject group included 15 children, 5-12 years old, with brain injury incurred prior to the age of 2 months; 12 had left hemisphere (LH) damage and 3 had right hemisphere (RH) damage. The comparison group included 141 children, 5-10 years old, who were at the appropriate grade for age. The task required children to listen to sentences composed of two noun phrases (N) that varied in terms of animacy and a verb phrase (V) and then to indicate the agent of the action. Three word orders were presented: NVN, VNN, and NNV. Measures included the proportion of trials in which the first noun was selected (choice) and reaction time. Word order and animacy significantly influenced choice. The effect of subject group approached significance for choice. Word order and age influenced reaction time. The children with LH injury and two children with RH injury showed a developmental delay in choosing the appropriate N as agent; one child with RH injury had mature responses. The overlapping performance of children with LH and RH injury suggests that delays in the development of sentence comprehension strategies are more likely related to reliance on a smaller than usual neural network rather than to congenital specialization of the LH.

[1]  Otto Muzik,et al.  Brain Organization of Language after Early Unilateral Lesion: A PET Study , 1998, Brain and Language.

[2]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  From first words to grammar in children with focal brain injury , 1997 .

[3]  M. Just,et al.  Brain Activation Modulated by Sentence Comprehension , 1996, Science.

[4]  Susan A. Sullivan,et al.  Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised , 1998 .

[5]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  A maximum likelihood procedure for the analysis of group and individual data in aphasia research , 1991, Brain and Language.

[6]  M. Dennis,et al.  Comprehension of syntax in infantile hemiplegics after cerebral hemidecortication: Left-hemisphere superiority , 1975, Brain and Language.

[7]  Julia L. Evans,et al.  Sentence processing strategies in children with expressive and expressive-receptive specific language impairments. , 1999, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[8]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  Neural mediation of language development: Perspectives from lesion studies of infants and children , 1999 .

[9]  C. Nelson,et al.  Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .

[10]  S. Pinker The Language Instinct , 1994 .

[11]  D. Aram,et al.  Comprehension and Imitation of Syntax Following Early Hemisphere Damage , 1994, Brain and Language.

[12]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Online Measures of Basic Language Skills in Children with Early Focal Brain Lesions , 2000, Brain and Language.

[13]  Cleborne D. Maddux,et al.  Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III) , 1999 .

[14]  Virginia A. Marchman,et al.  Narrative Discourse in Children with Early Focal Brain Injury , 2022 .

[15]  M. Dennis,et al.  Language acquisition following hemidecortication: Linguistic superiority of the left over the right hemisphere , 1976, Brain and Language.

[16]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Developmental and Lesion Effects in Brain Activation During Sentence Comprehension and Mental Rotation , 2000, Developmental neuropsychology.

[17]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[18]  D. Bulas,et al.  Pattern of Neuropsychological Deficit at Age Five Years Following Neonatal Unilateral Brain Injury , 1998, Brain and Language.

[19]  F. Vargha-Khadem,et al.  Development of speech and language following bilateral frontal lesions , 1985, Brain and Language.

[20]  D. Aram,et al.  Unilateral brain lesions in childhood: Performance on the revised token test , 1987, Brain and Language.

[21]  S. Carey,et al.  Language deficits after apparent clinical recovery from childhood aphasia , 1979, Annals of neurology.

[22]  D. Bishop,et al.  Linguistic Impairment after Left Hemidecortication for Infantile Hemiplegia? A Reappraisal , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[23]  M. Dennis,et al.  Written language acquisition after left or right hemidecortication in infancy , 1981, Brain and Language.

[24]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. , 1992 .

[25]  F. Vargha-Khadem,et al.  A review of cognitive outcome after hemidecortication in humans. , 1992, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[26]  E. Bates,et al.  EARLY COGNITION, COMMUNICATION AND LANGUAGE IN CHILDREN WITH FOCAL BRAIN INJURY , 1994, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[27]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  Babble and first words in children with focal brain injury , 1991, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[28]  Maureen Dennis,et al.  Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemidecortication , 1980, Brain and Language.

[29]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT IN CHILDREN WITH UNILATERAL BRAIN INJURY , 2000 .

[30]  R. Stark,et al.  Follow-up Study of a Right- and a Left-Hemispherectomized Child: Implications for Localization and Impairment of Language in Children , 1997, Brain and Language.

[31]  Changing patterns of childhood aphasia. , 1978, Annals of neurology.

[32]  V. Marchman Constraints on Plasticity in a Connectionist Model of the English Past Tense , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Janine E. Janosky,et al.  Language development after unilateral brain injury , 1992, Brain and Language.

[34]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[35]  J. A. Shafer,et al.  Understanding aphasia. , 1954, Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation.

[36]  F. Dick,et al.  Interpretation of complex syntax in aphasic adults and children with focal lesions or specific language impairment. , 1999 .

[37]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Sentence comprehension in aphasia in two clear case-marking languages , 1991, Brain and Language.

[38]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Onset of speech after left hemispherectomy in a nine-year-old boy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Maureen Dennis,et al.  Impaired sensory and motor differentiation with corpus callosum agenesis: A lack of callosal inhibition during ontogeny? , 1976, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  E. Bates,et al.  Developmental changes in real-time sentence processing , 1996 .

[41]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[42]  Dorothy M. Aram,et al.  Language ability in right and left hemiplegic children , 1981, Brain and Language.

[43]  James R. Booth,et al.  Functional organization of activation patterns in children: Whole brain fMRI imaging during three different cognitive tasks , 1999, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[44]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Functional anatomy of cognitive development , 2000, Neurology.

[45]  R. Müller,et al.  Language organization in patients with early and lateleft-hemisphere lesion: a pet study , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  N. Tzourio,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Dominance for Speech Comprehension in Left Handers vs Right Handers , 1998, NeuroImage.

[47]  H. Whitaker,et al.  Verbal and cognitive sequelae following unilateral lesions acquired in early childhood. , 1985, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[48]  F. Dick,et al.  POSTER SESSION 2: LANGUAGE COMPREHENSION, PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING, PRODUCTION, AND LATERALITY , 1999, Brain and Language.

[49]  H. Whitaker,et al.  Spoken syntax in children with acquired unilateral hemisphere lesions , 1986, Brain and Language.

[50]  B. Banker,et al.  Periventricular leukomalacia of infancy. A form of neonatal anoxic encephalopathy. , 1962, Archives of neurology.

[51]  E. Bates Origins of language disorders: A comparative approach , 1997 .

[52]  B MacWhinney,et al.  A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. , 1984, Child development.

[53]  E. Lenneberg Biological Foundations of Language , 1967 .

[54]  Faraneh Vargha-Khadem,et al.  A Review of Cognitive Outcome After Unilateral Lesions Sustained During Childhood , 1994, Journal of child neurology.

[55]  B Milner,et al.  THE ROLE OF EARLY LEFT‐BRAIN INJURY IN DETERMINING LATERALIZATION OF CEREBRAL SPEECH FUNCTIONS , 1977, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[56]  M. Denckla,et al.  EVIDENCE FOR DIFFERENTIAL HEMISPHERIC FUNCTION IN CHILDREN WITH HEMIPLEGIC CEREBRAL PALSY , 1983, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[57]  E. Bates,et al.  Early lexical development in children with focal brain injury , 1991, Brain and Language.

[58]  D. Aram,et al.  Fluency of conversational speech in children with unilateral brain lesions , 1990, Brain and Language.

[59]  Helen Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Constraints on Language Acquisition : Studies of Atypical Children , 1996 .

[60]  J. Brandt,et al.  Speech-Language Outcomes of Hemispherectomy in Children and Young Adults , 1995, Brain and Language.