Towards a Paradigm for Research on Social Representations

Based on Moscovici’s (1961) classical study on the cultivation of psychoanalytic ideas in France in the 1950’s and our own research on modern biotechnology, we propose a paradigm for researching social representations. Following a consideration of the nature of representations and of the ‘iconoclastic suspicion’ that haunts them, we propose a model of the emergence of meaning relating three elements: subjects, objects, and projects. The basic unit of analysis is the elongated triangle of mediation (SOPS): subject 1, object, project, and subject 2, captured in the image of a ‘Toblerone’. Such social units cultivate, that is produce, circulate and receive representation which may be embodied in four modes–habitual behaviour, individual cognition, informal communication and formal communication–and in three mediums–words, visual images or non-linguistic sounds. We propose an operational definition of a ‘social representation’ as the comparison of four characteristics of communication systems: the content structures (anchorings and objectifications; core and peripheral elements), the typified processes (diffusion, propagation, propaganda etc.), and their functions (identity, attitude, opinion, resistance, ideology etc.), within the context of segmented social milieus. Seven implications for research on social representations are outlined: (1) content and process; (2) segmentation by social milieus rather than taxonomies; (3) cultivation studies within social milieus; (4) multi-method (mode and medium) analysis; (5) time structures and longitudinal data; (6) the crossover of cultural projects and trajectories; (7) the disinterested research attitude. This ideal type paradigm leads to an operational clarification to identify new research questions, and to guide the design and evaluation of studies on social representations.

[1]  Stephen Hilgartner,et al.  The Dominant View of Popularization: Conceptual Problems, Political Uses , 1990 .

[2]  P. Crone Islam, Judeo-Christianity and Byzantine Iconoclasm , 1980 .

[3]  Frederick J. McGinness,et al.  : War against the Idols: The Reformation of Worship from Erasmus to Calvin , 1988 .

[4]  N. Tsouyopoulos Auf der Suche nach einer adäquaten Methode für die Geschichte und Theorie der Medizin. Auseinandersetzung mit Ludwik Flecks "Entstehung und Entwicklung einer wissenschaftlichen Tatsache". , 1982, Medizinhistorisches Journal.

[5]  E. H. Witte,et al.  Small group processes and interpersonal relations , 1996 .

[6]  Martin Bauer,et al.  The medicalization of science news - from the “rocket-scalpel” to the “gene-meteorite” complex , 1998 .

[7]  Renaud Dulong,et al.  Popularisation of science for adults , 1972 .

[8]  Alain Clémence,et al.  The Quantitative Analysis Of Social Representations , 1993 .

[9]  A. Bangerter Rethinking the relation between science and common sense: a comment on the current state of social representation theory , 1995 .

[10]  Baudouin Jurdant Popularization of science as the autobiography of science , 1993 .

[11]  D. Weatherall The Troubled Helix: Social and Psychological Implications of the New Human Genetics , 1996 .

[12]  R. Farr,et al.  Common sense, science and social representations , 1993 .

[13]  R. Farr,et al.  Social representations of the individual: A post-Communist perspective , 1998 .

[14]  M. Vovelle Ideologies and Mentalities , 1990 .

[15]  George Gaskell,et al.  The Social Psychological Study of Widespread Beliefs , 1990 .

[16]  Hans Blumenberg,et al.  Work on myth , 1985 .

[17]  The context of social psychology , 1972 .

[18]  SURVEY RESEARCH AND CONSENSUALITY: STATISTICAL AND NATURAL GROUPS , 1994 .

[19]  Mario De Caro,et al.  Recensione di Mark Pickering (ed.), Science as Practice and Culture , 1995 .

[20]  G. Gaskell,et al.  Two cultures of public understanding of science and technology in Europe , 2000 .

[21]  Kurt Lewin,et al.  Behavior and development as a function of the total situation. , 1946 .

[22]  Uwe Flick,et al.  The psychology of the social , 1998 .

[23]  R. Farr Social Representations: A French Tradition of Research. , 1987 .

[24]  Sheila Jasanoff,et al.  Handbook of Science and Technology Studies , 1995 .

[25]  A. Kolnai Rationalism in Politics , 1965, Philosophy.

[26]  H. Cantril,et al.  They saw a game: a case study. , 2011, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[27]  G. Duveen,et al.  Madness and social representations , 1991 .

[28]  Martin W. Bauer,et al.  Europe ambivalent on biotechnology , 1997, Nature.

[29]  Martin W. Bauer,et al.  Resistance to new technology: Towards a functional analysis of resistance , 1995 .

[30]  A. Markman,et al.  In Defense of Representation , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  R. Tuomela The Importance of Us: A Philosophical Study of Basic Social Notions , 1995 .

[32]  E. A. Stead,et al.  Health and Illness , 1997 .

[33]  William A. Hatt,et al.  Nuclear fear: A history of images , 1989 .

[34]  J. Forgas Social Cognition: Perspectives on Everyday Understanding , 1982 .

[35]  Wolfgang Wagner,et al.  Theory and method of social representations , 1999 .

[36]  C. Dornan,et al.  Some problems in conceptualizing the issue of “science and the media” , 1990 .

[37]  G. Duveen The Psychosocial Production of Ideas: Social Representations and Psychologic , 1998 .

[38]  B. Wynne,et al.  Misunderstanding science? : the public reconstruction of science and technology , 1996 .

[39]  Wolf Lepenies Melancholie und Gesellschaft , 1972 .

[40]  T. Marteau,et al.  THE TROUBLED HELIX: SOCIAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF THE NEW HUMAN GENETICS , 1996 .

[41]  Saadi Lahlou,et al.  Functional aspects of social representations , 2001 .

[42]  Meinolf Dierkes,et al.  Between Understanding and Trust: The Public, Science and Technology , 1999 .

[43]  J. Durant,et al.  The public understanding of science , 1989, Nature.

[44]  A. Touraine The crisis of progress , 1993 .

[45]  Uwe Flick,et al.  Triangulation Revisited: Strategy of Validation or Alternative? , 1992 .

[46]  A. Pickering Science as practice and culture , 1992 .