Dorsal hippocampus contributes to model-based planning
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Jiqiang Guo,et al. Stan: A Probabilistic Programming Language. , 2017, Journal of statistical software.
[2] Matthijs Verhage,et al. A solution to dependency: using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.
[3] J. O’Keefe,et al. Space in the brain: how the hippocampal formation supports spatial cognition , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[4] P. Dayan,et al. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[5] Nic Wilson,et al. A Probabilistic Programming Language for Influence Diagrams , 2017, SUM.
[6] David B. Dunson,et al. Bayesian Data Analysis , 2010 .
[7] Robert C. Wilson,et al. Orbitofrontal Cortex as a Cognitive Map of Task Space , 2014, Neuron.
[8] Robert Colin Honey,et al. Excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampus leave sensory preconditioning intact: Implications for models of hippocampal function , 2001 .
[9] D. Hassabis,et al. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[10] Brad E. Pfeiffer,et al. Hippocampal place cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals , 2013, Nature.
[11] Y. Niv,et al. Ventral Striatum and Orbitofrontal Cortex Are Both Required for Model-Based, But Not Model-Free, Reinforcement Learning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] G. Handelmann,et al. Hippocampus, space, and memory , 1979 .
[13] Peter Dayan,et al. Simple Plans or Sophisticated Habits? State, Transition and Learning Interactions in the Two-step Task , 2015, bioRxiv.
[14] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[15] E. Tolman. Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.
[16] B. Balleine,et al. Sensitivity to Instrumental Contingency Degradation Is Mediated by the Entorhinal Cortex and Its Efferents via the Dorsal Hippocampus , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] D. Kimble,et al. Latent learning in hippocampal-lesioned rats , 1981, Physiology & Behavior.
[18] G. Schoenbaum,et al. What the orbitofrontal cortex does not do , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] R. Racine,et al. Hippocampal lesions and delayed alternation in the rat , 1965 .
[20] H. Eichenbaum,et al. Memory for the Order of Events in Specific Sequences: Contributions of the Hippocampus and Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] R. Costa,et al. Endocannabinoid Signaling is Critical for Habit Formation , 2007, Frontiers in integrative neuroscience.
[22] Matthijs A. A. van der Meer,et al. Internally generated sequences in learning and executing goal-directed behavior , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] Joshua L. Jones,et al. Orbitofrontal Cortex Supports Behavior and Learning Using Inferred But Not Cached Values , 2012, Science.
[24] G. Winocur,et al. Higher-Order Conditioning Is Impaired by Hippocampal Lesions , 2014, Current Biology.
[25] Carlos D. Brody,et al. Identifying Model-Based and Model-Free Patterns in Behavior on Multi-Step Tasks , 2016, bioRxiv.
[26] R. F. Thompson,et al. Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit's classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.
[27] David J. Foster,et al. Sequence learning and the role of the hippocampus in rodent navigation , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[28] Wouter Kool,et al. When Does Model-Based Control Pay Off? , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[29] H. Eichenbaum,et al. Can We Reconcile the Declarative Memory and Spatial Navigation Views on Hippocampal Function? , 2014, Neuron.
[30] David J. Foster,et al. A model of hippocampally dependent navigation, using the temporal difference learning rule , 2000, Hippocampus.
[31] Peter Dayan,et al. Interplay of approximate planning strategies , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[32] W. Brogden. Sensory pre-conditioning. , 1939 .
[33] B. Balleine,et al. Human and Rodent Homologies in Action Control: Corticostriatal Determinants of Goal-Directed and Habitual Action , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[34] Christopher D. Adams,et al. Instrumental Responding following Reinforcer Devaluation , 1981 .
[35] R. Buckner. The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. , 2010, Annual review of psychology.
[36] R. C. Honey,et al. Excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampus leave sensory preconditioning intact: implications for models of hippocampal function. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.
[37] P. Dudchenko. The hippocampus as a cognitive map , 2010 .
[38] H. Eichenbaum,et al. The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[39] S. Duane,et al. Hybrid Monte Carlo , 1987 .
[40] D. Kimble,et al. Further evidence for latent learning in hippocampal-lesioned rats , 1982, Physiology & Behavior.
[41] C. Padoa-Schioppa. Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.
[42] P. Dayan,et al. Goals and Habits in the Brain , 2013, Neuron.
[43] P. Dayan,et al. Mapping value based planning and extensively trained choice in the human brain , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[44] H. Eichenbaum,et al. Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans , 1996, Nature.
[45] N. White,et al. Inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus does not affect learning during exploration of a novel environment , 2005, Hippocampus.
[46] P. Dayan,et al. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors , 2011, Neuron.
[47] B. Balleine,et al. Orbitofrontal Cortex Mediates Outcome Encoding in Pavlovian But Not Instrumental Conditioning , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[48] John K Kruschke,et al. Bayesian data analysis. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[49] L. Nadel,et al. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .
[50] R. Morris,et al. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.
[51] M. Nicolelis,et al. Immediate thalamic sensory plasticity depends on corticothalamic feedback. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[52] Zeb Kurth-Nelson,et al. Model-Based Reasoning in Humans Becomes Automatic with Training , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[53] Guido Bugmann,et al. Effects of medial septal lesions: implications for models of hippocampal function , 1997 .
[54] B. Balleine,et al. The Role of the Hippocampus in Instrumental Conditioning , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[55] J. D. McGaugh,et al. Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[56] Andrew M. Wikenheiser,et al. Hippocampal theta sequences reflect current goals , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.
[57] T. Robbins,et al. Decision Making, Affect, and Learning: Attention and Performance XXIII , 2011 .
[58] L. Jarrard,et al. Contributions of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to energy and body weight regulation , 2009, Hippocampus.
[59] J. O'Keefe,et al. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.
[60] Michael E. Hasselmo,et al. Modeling goal-directed spatial navigation in the rat based on physiological data from the hippocampal formation , 2003, Neural Networks.
[61] P. Glimcher,et al. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 84, 555–579 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) DYNAMIC RESPONSE-BY-RESPONSE MODELS OF MATCHING BEHAVIOR IN RHESUS MONKEYS , 2022 .
[62] John H. Martin. Autoradiographic estimation of the extent of reversible inactivation produced by microinjection of lidocaine and muscimol in the rat , 1991, Neuroscience Letters.
[63] Dylan A. Simon,et al. Neural Correlates of Forward Planning in a Spatial Decision Task in Humans , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[64] Amir Dezfouli,et al. Speed/Accuracy Trade-Off between the Habitual and the Goal-Directed Processes , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[65] R. Costa,et al. Orbitofrontal and striatal circuits dynamically encode the shift between goal-directed and habitual actions , 2013, Nature Communications.