Haptic discrimination of object shape in humans: two-dimensional angle discrimination

The human ability to recognize objects on the basis of their shape, as defined by active exploratory movements, is dependent on sensory feedback from mechanoreceptors located both in the skin and in deep structures (haptic feedback). Surprisingly, we have little information about the mechanisms for integrating these different signals into a single sensory percept. With the eventual aim of studying the underlying central neural mechanisms, we developed a shape discrimination test that required active exploration of objects, but was restricted to one component of shape, two-dimensional (2D) angles. The angles were machined from 1-cm-thick Plexiglas, and consisted of two 8-cm-long arms that met to form an angle of 90° (standard) or 91° to 103° (comparison angles). Subjects scanned pairs of angles with the index finger of the outstretched arm and identified the larger angle of each pair explored. Discrimination threshold (75% correct) was 4.7° (range 0.7° to 12.1°), giving a precision of 5.2% (0.8–13.4%: difference/standard). Repeated blocks of trials, either in the same session or on different days, had no effect on discrimination threshold. In contrast, the motor strategy was partly modified: scanning speed increased but dwell-time at the intersection did not change. Finally, 2D angle discrimination was not significantly modified by rotating the orientation of one of the angles in the pair (0°, 4° or 8° rotation towards the midline, in the vertical plane), providing evidence that subjects evaluated each angle independently in each trial. Subject reports indicated that they relied on cutaneous feedback from the exploring digit (amount of compression of the finger at the angle) and mental images of the angles, most likely arising from proprioceptive information (from the shoulder) generated during the to-and-fro scans over the angle. In terms of shoulder angles, the mean discrimination threshold here was 0.54° (range 0.08° to 1.36°). These values are lower than previous estimates of position sense at the shoulder. In light of the subjects’ strategies, it therefore seems likely that both cutaneous and proprioceptive (including both dynamic and static position-related signals) feedback contributed to the haptic discrimination of 2D angles.

[1]  L. Cohen,et al.  Analysis of position sense in human shoulder. , 1958, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[3]  J. I. Laszlo,et al.  The Measurement of Kinaesthetic Sensitivity in Children and Adults , 1980, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[4]  K O Johnson,et al.  Sensory discrimination: decision process. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Does position sense at the elbow reflect a sense of elbow joint angle or one of limb orientation? , 1982, Brain Research.

[6]  I. Gordon,et al.  The haptic perception of curvature , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Detections of movements imposed on finger, elbow and shoulder joints. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  Per E. Roland,et al.  Somatosensory detection of microgeometry, macrogeometry and kinesthesia in man , 1987, Brain Research Reviews.

[9]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Tactile discrimination of shape: responses of slowly adapting mechanoreceptor afferents to a step stroked across the monkey fingerpad , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Tactile discrimination of shape: responses of rapidly adapting mechanoreceptive afferents to a step stroked across the monkey fingerpad , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  R. J. Irwin,et al.  Differential thresholds for limb movement measured using adaptive techniques , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  F. J. Clark,et al.  HOW ACCURATELY CAN WE PERCEIVE THE POSITIONS OF OUR LIMBS , 1992 .

[14]  G E Loeb,et al.  The computation of position sense from spindles in mono- and multiarticular muscles , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  P Alvarez,et al.  The animal model of human amnesia: long-term memory impaired and short-term memory intact. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  A. Goodwin,et al.  Tactile resolution: peripheral neural mechanisms underlying the human capacity to determine positions of objects contacting the fingerpad , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  A. Goodwin,et al.  Encoding of object curvature by tactile afferents from human fingers. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  B. Weder,et al.  Somatosensory discrimination of shape: prediction of success in normal volunteers and parkinsonian patients , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Y Agid,et al.  Temporal limits of spatial working memory in humans , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[20]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  M. Petrides Dissociable Roles of Mid-Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex in Visual Working Memory , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Relative effects of the spatial and temporal characteristics of scanned surfaces on human perception of tactile roughness using passive touch , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Haptic interaction with virtual objects. Spatial perception and motor control. , 2000, Biological cybernetics.

[24]  M. Petrides The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Movement preparation and working memory: a behavioural dissociation , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Haptic discrimination of object shape in humans: contribution of cutaneous and proprioceptive inputs , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  F. J. Clark,et al.  A metric for assessing acuity in positioning joints and limbs , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  W. G. Darling,et al.  Perception of forearm angles in 3-dimensional space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  A. Goodwin,et al.  Tactile discrimination of curvature by humans using only cutaneous information from the fingerpads , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.