Auditory distraction: A duplex-mechanism account.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. Frith,et al. The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.
[2] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Privileged Access by Irrelevant Speech to Short-term Memory: The Role of Changing State , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[3] R. Engle. Working Memory and Retrieval: An Inhibition-Resource Approach , 1996 .
[4] R. Engle,et al. Is working memory capacity task dependent , 1989 .
[5] Peng Yuan,et al. The effect of visuospatial attentional load on the processing of irrelevant acoustic distractors , 2006, NeuroImage.
[6] Ian Neath,et al. Directly Assessing the Relationship between Irrelevant Speech and Articulatory Suppression , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[7] Dylan M. Jones,et al. The phonological store of working memory: is it phonological and is it a store? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[8] Patrik Sörqvist,et al. High working memory capacity does not always attenuate distraction: Bayesian evidence in support of the null hypothesis , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[9] Dylan M. Jones,et al. What causes auditory distraction? , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[10] Hanna Benoni,et al. Conceptual and methodological concerns in the theory of perceptual load , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[11] R. Knight,et al. Neural Mechanisms of Involuntary Attention to Acoustic Novelty and Change , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Cognitive control of auditory distraction: impact of task difficulty, foreknowledge, and working memory capacity supports duplex-mechanism account. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Organizational factors in the effect of irrelevant speech: The role of spatial location and timing , 1995, Memory & cognition.
[14] C Philip Beaman,et al. The irrelevant sound phenomenon revisited: what role for working memory capacity? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[15] T Fernández,et al. Primary task demands modulate P3a amplitude. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[16] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect : Implications for phonological coding in working memory , 1993 .
[17] N. Moray. Attention in Dichotic Listening: Affective Cues and the Influence of Instructions , 1959 .
[18] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Phonological similarity in the irrelevant speech effect: Within- or between-stream similarity? , 1995 .
[19] J. Hanley,et al. Does articulatory suppression remove the irrelevant speech effect? , 1997, Memory.
[20] Thomas S. Redick,et al. Working memory capacity and go/no-go task performance: selective effects of updating, maintenance, and inhibition. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[21] Erich Schröger,et al. Working memory controls involuntary attention switching: evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[22] Andrew R. A. Conway,et al. A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[23] C. Escera,et al. The cognitive locus of distraction by acoustic novelty in the cross-modal oddball task , 2008, Cognition.
[24] Dylan M. Jones,et al. The impact of order incongruence between a task-irrelevant auditory sequence and a task-relevant visual sequence. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Explaining the irrelevant-sound effect: Temporal distinctiveness or changing state? , 1999 .
[26] Patrik Sörqvist,et al. High working memory capacity attenuates the deviation effect but not the changing-state effect: Further support for the duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction , 2010, Memory & cognition.
[27] Alan B. Welsh,et al. Acoustic masking in primary memory. , 1976 .
[28] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Retrieval from memory: vulnerable or inviolable? , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[29] D. Ravenzwaaij,et al. Individual differences in conflict-monitoring: testing means and covariance hypothesis about the Simon and the Eriksen Flanker task , 2009, Psychological research.
[30] E. Elliott,et al. Investigating the role of attentional resources in the irrelevant speech effect. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[31] I. Winkler,et al. Preventing distraction: Assessing stimulus-specific and general effects of the predictive cueing of deviant auditory events , 2011, Biological Psychology.
[32] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[33] A. Muller-Gass,et al. The effect of visual task difficulty and attentional direction on the detection of acoustic change as indexed by the Mismatch Negativity , 2006, Brain Research.
[34] A. Buchner,et al. The role of habituation and attentional orienting in the disruption of short-term memory performance , 2011, Memory & cognition.
[35] E. Schröger,et al. Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[36] J. Driver,et al. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .
[37] Jonathan T. Mall,et al. Goal-neglect links Stroop interference with working memory capacity. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[38] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Auditory Distraction in Semantic Memory: A Process-Based Approach. , 2008 .
[39] Thomas Lachmann,et al. The irrelevant sound effect in short-term memory: Is there developmental change? , 2010 .
[40] R. Engle. Working Memory Capacity as Executive Attention , 2002 .
[41] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Organizational factors in selective attention: The interplay of acoustic distinctiveness and auditory streaming in the irrelevant sound effect , 1999 .
[42] The effects of irrelevant speech and articulatory suppression on the serial recall of silently presented lipread digits. , 2001, British journal of psychology.
[43] A. Buchner,et al. Self-relevance increases the irrelevant sound effect: Attentional disruption by one's own name , 2013 .
[44] R. Engle,et al. Executive Attention, Working Memory Capacity, and a Two-Factor Theory of Cognitive Control. , 2003 .
[45] O. Neumann. Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention , 1987 .
[46] Mattias Holmgren,et al. Effects of speech on proofreading: can task-engagement manipulations shield against distraction? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[47] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Inhibitory control in memory: evidence for negative priming in free recall. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[48] Nelson Cowan,et al. Coherence of the irrelevant-sound effect: Individual profiles of short-term memory and susceptibility to task-irrelevant materials , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[49] Ian Neath,et al. Irrelevant speech effects and sequence learning , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[50] Elke B. Lange. Disruption of attention by irrelevant stimuli in serial recall , 2005 .
[51] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[52] N. Lavie. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[53] Alan D. Baddeley,et al. Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech : Implications for the structure of working memory , 1982 .
[54] Carles Escera,et al. When Loading Working Memory Reduces Distraction: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence from an Auditory-Visual Distraction Paradigm , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[55] F. Parmentier. Towards a cognitive model of distraction by auditory novelty: The role of involuntary attention capture and semantic processing , 2008, Cognition.
[56] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Disruption by speech of serial short-term memory: The role of changing-state vowels , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[57] Axel Buchner,et al. Age-related differences in irrelevant-speech effects. , 2008, Psychology and aging.
[58] R. Näätänen. The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[59] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Auditory attentional capture during serial recall: violations at encoding of an algorithm-based neural model? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[60] Nilli Lavie,et al. Contrasting effects of sensory limits and capacity limits in visual selective attention , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[61] D. Linden,et al. Attention capture by novel sounds: Distraction versus facilitation , 2010 .
[62] Michael F. Bunting,et al. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[63] I. Winkler,et al. Top-down control over involuntary attention switching in the auditory modality , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[64] Y. Tsal,et al. Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[65] E. Elliott,et al. The irrelevant-speech effect and children: theoretical implications of developmental change , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[66] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Disruption of short-term memory by changing and deviant sounds: support for a duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[67] Bill Macken. Auditory distraction and perceptual organization: Streams of unconscious processing. , 2014, PsyCh journal.
[68] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Interference by process, not content, determines semantic auditory distraction , 2009, Cognition.
[69] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. On the Irrelevance of Semantic Information for the “Irrelevant Speech” Effect , 1996 .
[70] S. T. Klapp,et al. Short-term memory does not involve the "working memory" of information processing: The demise of a common assumption , 1983 .
[71] I. Winkler,et al. The role of attention in the formation of auditory streams , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[72] E. Viding,et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[73] N. Cowan,et al. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: how frequent are attention shifts to one's name in an irrelevant auditory channel? , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[74] I. Neath. Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[75] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory , and Cognition Broken Expectations : Violation of Expectancies , Not Novelty , Captures Auditory Attention , 2011 .
[76] E. Elliott,et al. The Distracting Effects of a Ringing Cell Phone: An Investigation of the Laboratory and the Classroom Setting. , 2009, Journal of environmental psychology.
[77] HERMAN BUSCHKE,et al. Relative Retention in Immediate Memory determined by the Missing Scan Method , 1963, Nature.
[78] N. Lavie. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[79] Rebecca A. Grier,et al. Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: task engagement, distress, and worry. , 2002, Emotion.
[80] B. Murdock. Serial order effects in short-term memory. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[81] Albert S. Bregman,et al. The Auditory Scene. (Book Reviews: Auditory Scene Analysis. The Perceptual Organization of Sound.) , 1990 .
[82] P. Carpenter,et al. Individual differences in working memory and reading , 1980 .
[83] Andrew R. A. Conway,et al. On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.
[84] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Role of Serial Order in the Irrelevant Speech Effect: Tests of the Changing-State Hypothesis , 1997 .
[85] N. Cowan. Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .
[86] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Cross-modal distraction by background speech: what role for meaning? , 2010, Noise & health.
[87] W. Ellermeier,et al. Individual differences in susceptibility to the "irrelevant speech effect". , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[88] R. Engle,et al. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[89] Gene A. Brewer,et al. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Variation in Working Memory Capacity and Cognitive Control: Goal Maintenance and Microadjustments of Control , 2022 .
[90] Michael D. Dodd,et al. In Opposition to Inhibition , 2003 .