Predicting task-general mind-wandering with EEG

Mind-wandering refers to the process of thinking task-unrelated thoughts while performing a task. The dynamics of mind-wandering remain elusive because it is difficult to track when someone’s mind is wandering based only on behavior. The goal of this study is to develop a machine-learning classifier that can determine someone’s mind-wandering state online using electroencephalography (EEG) in a way that generalizes across tasks. In particular, we trained machine-learning models on EEG markers to classify the participants’ current state as either mind-wandering or on-task. To be able to examine the task generality of the classifier, two different paradigms were adopted in this study: a sustained attention to response task (SART) and a visual search task. In both tasks, probe questions asking for a self-report of the thoughts at that moment were inserted at random moments, and participants’ responses to the probes were used to create labels for the classifier. The 6 trials preceding an off-task response were labeled as mind-wandering, whereas the 6 trials predicting an on-task response were labeled as on-task. The EEG markers used as features for the classifier included single-trial P1, N1, and P3, the power and coherence in the theta (4–8 Hz) and alpha (8.5–12 Hz) bands at PO7, Pz, PO8, and Fz. We used a support vector machine as the training algorithm to learn the connection between EEG markers and the current mind-wandering state. We were able to distinguish between on-task and off-task thinking with an accuracy ranging from 0.50 to 0.85. Moreover, the classifiers were task-general: The average accuracy in across-task prediction was 60%, which was above chance level. Among all the extracted EEG markers, alpha power was most predictive of mind-wandering.

[1]  M Congedo,et al.  A review of classification algorithms for EEG-based brain–computer interfaces , 2007, Journal of neural engineering.

[2]  Todd C. Handy,et al.  The neurocognitive consequences of the wandering mind: a mechanistic account of sensory-motor decoupling , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[3]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[4]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[5]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Working memory capacity and mind-wandering during low-demand cognitive tasks , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[6]  Mikaël Bastian,et al.  Mind wandering at the fingertips: automatic parsing of subjective states based on response time variability , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[7]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.

[8]  K.-R. Muller,et al.  Optimizing Spatial filters for Robust EEG Single-Trial Analysis , 2008, IEEE Signal Processing Magazine.

[9]  Michael J. Kane,et al.  Dispatching the wandering mind? Toward a laboratory method for cuing “spontaneous” off-task thought , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[10]  Lawrence M. Ward,et al.  Human Neuroscience Original Research Article Differential Synchronization in Default and Task-specific Networks of the Human Brain , 2022 .

[11]  Andrew P. Robinson,et al.  Model validation using equivalence tests , 2004 .

[12]  D. Smilek,et al.  On the Necessity of Distinguishing Between Unintentional and Intentional Mind Wandering , 2016, Psychological science.

[13]  Evelyn Barron,et al.  Absorbed in Thought , 2011, Psychological science.

[14]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Working Memory Capacity Offers Resistance to Mind‐Wandering and External Distraction in a Context‐Specific Manner , 2015 .

[15]  Daniel M. Wegner,et al.  Mind-blanking: when the mind goes away , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[16]  K. Christoff,et al.  Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[18]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Nitesh V. Chawla,et al.  Data Mining for Imbalanced Datasets: An Overview , 2005, The Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery Handbook.

[21]  F. Castellanos,et al.  Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: A neurobiological hypothesis , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  Zachary C. Irving,et al.  Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  W. Iacono,et al.  Theta- and delta-band EEG network dynamics during a novelty oddball task. , 2017, Psychophysiology.

[24]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Broadband changes in the cortical surface potential track activation of functionally diverse neuronal populations , 2014, NeuroImage.

[25]  Vladimir Bostanov,et al.  The t-CWT: A new ERP detection and quantification method based on the continuous wavelet transform and Student’s t-statistics , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[26]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Localizing visual discrimination processes in time and space. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  T. Ergenoğlu,et al.  Alpha rhythm of the EEG modulates visual detection performance in humans. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[28]  Kevin Fitzpatrick,et al.  Slow Fluctuations in Attentional Control of Sensory Cortex , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The restless mind. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[31]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Marieke K. van Vugt,et al.  The wandering self: Tracking distracting self-generated thought in a cognitively demanding context , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[33]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  When the Brain Takes a Break: A Model-Based Analysis of Mind Wandering , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Kane,et al.  Conducting the train of thought: working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  Jorge Leite,et al.  Mind Wandering and Task-Focused Attention: ERP Correlates , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[36]  M. Frank,et al.  Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Going AWOL in the Brain: Mind Wandering Reduces Cortical Analysis of External Events , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  James S. P. Macdonald,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Variations in Subjective Attentional State are Reflected in Ongoing Prestimulus EEG Alpha Oscillations , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[39]  Vladimir Bostanov,et al.  BCI competition 2003-data sets Ib and IIb: feature extraction from event-related brain potentials with the continuous wavelet transform and the t-value scalogram , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[40]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Stages of Processing in Associative Recognition: Evidence from Behavior, EEG, and Classification , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Nico Broers,et al.  Self-Reported Stickiness of Mind-Wandering Affects Task Performance , 2016, Front. Psychol..