A general theory of intertemporal decision-making and the perception of time

Animals and humans make decisions based on their expected outcomes. Since relevant outcomes are often delayed, perceiving delays and choosing between earlier vs. later rewards (intertemporal decision-making) is an essential component of animal behavior. The myriad observations made in experiments studying intertemporal decision-making and time perception have not yet been rationalized within a single theory. Here we present a theory—Training-Integrated Maximized Estimation of Reinforcement Rate (TIMERR)—that explains a wide variety of behavioral observations made in intertemporal decision-making and the perception of time. Our theory postulates that animals make intertemporal choices to optimize expected reward rates over a limited temporal window which includes a past integration interval—over which experienced reward rate is estimated—as well as the expected delay to future reward. Using this theory, we derive mathematical expressions for both the subjective value of a delayed reward and the subjective representation of the delay. A unique contribution of our work is in finding that the past integration interval directly determines the steepness of temporal discounting and the non-linearity of time perception. In so doing, our theory provides a single framework to understand both intertemporal decision-making and time perception.

[1]  N. Davies,et al.  Behavioral ecology. , 1981, Science.

[2]  T. Bonhoeffer,et al.  Current opinion in neurobiology , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[3]  Benjamin Y. Hayden,et al.  Explicit Information Reduces Discounting Behavior in Monkeys , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[4]  Yael Niv,et al.  Operant Conditioning , 1971 .

[5]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[6]  John Mann The Frontiers of Psychology , 1935, Nature.

[7]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Optimal Temporal Risk Assessment , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[9]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[10]  HighWire Press The journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. , 1981 .

[11]  Redaktionen THE REVIEW OF ECONOMIC STUDIES , 1960 .

[12]  K. Kraay,et al.  The failure , 2020, Trust in Divided Societies.

[13]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Towards a general model of temporal discounting. , 2013, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[14]  Current Biology , 2012, Current Biology.

[15]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Neural Dissociation of Delay and Uncertainty in Intertemporal Choice , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Foraging , 2012, Science.

[17]  S. Patek,et al.  Integrative and Comparative Biology , 2006 .

[18]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Postreward delays and systematic biases in measures of animal temporal discounting , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Interval Timing by Long-Range Temporal Integration , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[20]  Paul F. Hill,et al.  Remember the Future: Working Memory Training Decreases Delay Discounting Among Stimulant Addicts , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[21]  P. Samuelson A Note on Measurement of Utility , 1937 .

[22]  Noah A. Shamosh,et al.  Individual Differences in Delay Discounting , 2008, Psychological science.

[23]  Miao‐kun Sun,et al.  Trends in cognitive sciences , 2012 .

[24]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Risky Theories—The Effects of Variance on Foraging Decisions , 1996 .

[25]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  G. Thines,et al.  Behavioural processes , 1976, Behavioural Processes.

[27]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Model of Interval Timing by Neural Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Evenden Varieties of impulsivity , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[29]  P. Dayan,et al.  Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[30]  Karin Ackermann,et al.  Progress In Neurobiology , 2016 .

[31]  A. Macić-Džanković,et al.  [Evaluation of risk]. , 1967, La Medicina del lavoro.

[32]  A. Odum,et al.  Impulsivity, risk taking, and timing , 2012, Behavioural Processes.

[33]  W. Meck,et al.  Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. , 2000, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[34]  W. Millard Species preferences of experimenters: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. , 1976 .

[35]  M. Lane,et al.  Towards a general model , 2002 .

[36]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[37]  R. Rosenfeld Nature , 2009, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[38]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Anomalies in Intertemporal Choice: Evidence and an Interpretation , 1992 .

[39]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  Mark F Bear,et al.  Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. , 2006, Science.

[41]  S. Tipper,et al.  Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.

[42]  D. Stephens,et al.  The adaptive value of preference for immediacy: when shortsighted rules have farsighted consequences , 2001 .

[43]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Rate currencies and the foraging starling: the fallacy of the averages revisited , 1996 .

[44]  J. Wearden,et al.  Scalar Properties in Human Timing: Conformity and Violations , 2006 .

[45]  C. Pennartz,et al.  Is a bird in the hand worth two in the future? The neuroeconomics of intertemporal decision-making , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[46]  P. Sherman,et al.  The Utility of the Proximate-Ultimate Dichotomy in Ethology , 2010 .

[47]  K. D. Valck,et al.  The Effect of Members' Satisfaction with a Virtual Community on Member Participation , 2003 .

[48]  Kenji Doya,et al.  Humans Can Adopt Optimal Discounting Strategy under Real-Time Constraints , 2006, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[49]  Robert S. Lockhart,et al.  Learning and Motivation , 1981 .

[50]  P. Killeen,et al.  A behavioral theory of timing. , 1988, Psychological review.

[51]  David S. Leland,et al.  Impaired time perception and motor timing in stimulant-dependent subjects. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[52]  B. Pitt Psychopharmacology , 1968, Mental Health.

[53]  Debajyoti Ray,et al.  Positive Temporal Dependence of the Biological Clock Implies Hyperbolic Discounting , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[54]  D. Stephens Decision ecology: Foraging and the ecology of animal decision making , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[55]  W. Schultz,et al.  Influence of Reward Delays on Responses of Dopamine Neurons , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  M. Lindauer Ethology. , 1962, Annual review of psychology.

[57]  Taiki Takahashi,et al.  Time-estimation error following Weber-Fechner law may explain subadditive time-discounting. , 2006, Medical hypotheses.

[58]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Human bisection at the geometric mean , 1991 .

[59]  M. Bateson,et al.  Single-trials analyses demonstrate that increases in clock speed contribute to the methamphetamine-induced horizontal shifts in peak-interval timing functions , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[60]  K. Doya Modulators of decision making , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[61]  David J. Hardisty,et al.  Good or Bad, We Want it Now: Fixed-Cost Present Bias for Gains and Losses Explains Magnitude Asymmetries in Intertemporal Choice , 2012 .

[62]  P. Worhunsky,et al.  Drug and Alcohol Dependence , 2017 .

[63]  M. Paulus,et al.  Decision making, impulsivity and time perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[64]  T. Kalenscher,et al.  Single Units in the Pigeon Brain Integrate Reward Amount and Time-to-Reward in an Impulsive Choice Task , 2005, Current Biology.

[65]  Lewis A. Bizo,et al.  Time's causes , 1997 .

[66]  Shanefrederick,et al.  Time Discounting and Time Preference : A Critical Review , 2022 .

[67]  R. Church,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and choice between delayed rewards. , 1988, Psychological review.

[68]  J. Wearden,et al.  Scalar properties in animal timing: conformity and violations. , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[69]  Josey Y. M. Chu,et al.  The failure of Weber's law in time perception and production , 2006, Behavioural Processes.

[70]  Jan Peters,et al.  The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[71]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review , 2002 .

[72]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[73]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.