Semantic representations in the temporal pole predict false memories

Significance False memories can arise in daily life through a mixture of factors, including misinformation and prior conceptual knowledge. This can have serious consequences in settings, such as legal eyewitness testimony, which depend on the accuracy of memory. We investigated the brain basis of false memory with fMRI, and found that patterns of activity in the temporal pole region of the brain can predict false memories. Furthermore, we show that each individual has unique patterns of brain activation that can predict their own idiosyncratic set of false-memory errors. Together, these results suggest that the temporal pole may be responsible for the conceptual component of illusory memories. Recent advances in neuroscience have given us unprecedented insight into the neural mechanisms of false memory, showing that artificial memories can be inserted into the memory cells of the hippocampus in a way that is indistinguishable from true memories. However, this alone is not enough to explain how false memories can arise naturally in the course of our daily lives. Cognitive psychology has demonstrated that many instances of false memory, both in the laboratory and the real world, can be attributed to semantic interference. Whereas previous studies have found that a diverse set of regions show some involvement in semantic false memory, none have revealed the nature of the semantic representations underpinning the phenomenon. Here we use fMRI with representational similarity analysis to search for a neural code consistent with semantic false memory. We find clear evidence that false memories emerge from a similarity-based neural code in the temporal pole, a region that has been called the “semantic hub” of the brain. We further show that each individual has a partially unique semantic code within the temporal pole, and this unique code can predict idiosyncratic patterns of memory errors. Finally, we show that the same neural code can also predict variation in true-memory performance, consistent with an adaptive perspective on false memory. Taken together, our findings reveal the underlying structure of neural representations of semantic knowledge, and how this semantic structure can both enhance and distort our memories.

[1]  J. Ojemann,et al.  Presurgical Language Mapping , 2005 .

[2]  Ingrid R. Olson,et al.  Converging evidence from fMRI and aphasia that the left temporoparietal cortex has an essential role in representing abstract semantic knowledge , 2015, Cortex.

[3]  J. Deese On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  L. Tyler,et al.  Understanding What We See: How We Derive Meaning From Vision , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[6]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Information-based functional brain mapping. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Susceptibility-Induced Loss of Signal: Comparing PET and fMRI on a Semantic Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[8]  R. Henson,et al.  How schema and novelty augment memory formation , 2012, Trends in Neurosciences.

[9]  K. McDermott,et al.  Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest , 2006, NeuroImage.

[11]  Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al.  Comparison of multivariate classifiers and response normalizations for pattern-information fMRI , 2010, NeuroImage.

[12]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex: compensation of susceptibility-induced gradients in the readout direction , 2007, Magnetic Resonance Materials in Physics, Biology and Medicine.

[13]  D. Balota,et al.  Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  Tom Michael Mitchell,et al.  Predicting Human Brain Activity Associated with the Meanings of Nouns , 2008, Science.

[15]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  CA3 size predicts the precision of memory recall , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  Henry L. Roediger,et al.  Spreading activation and arousal of false memories. , 2001 .

[17]  Gui Xue,et al.  Global Neural Pattern Similarity as a Common Basis for Categorization and Recognition Memory , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  H L Roediger,et al.  Norms for word lists that create false memories , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[19]  G. Fernández,et al.  Understanding Low Reliability of Memories for Neutral Information Encoded under Stress: Alterations in Memory-Related Activation in the Hippocampus and Midbrain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Temporal Lobe Cortical Electrical Stimulation during the Encoding and Retrieval Phase Reduces False Memories , 2009, PloS one.

[21]  T. Rogers,et al.  Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: A whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T , 2006, NeuroImage.

[23]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[24]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Memory Illusions: False Recall and Recognition in Amnesic Patients , 1996 .

[25]  Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al.  The Emergence of Semantic Meaning in the Ventral Temporal Pathway , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  False Recall Is Reduced by Damage to the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex: Implications for Understanding the Neural Correlates of Schematic Memory , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  S. Shinkareva,et al.  Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[28]  Allan Snyder,et al.  Reducing false memories by magnetic pulse stimulation , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[29]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  How Fuzzy-Trace Theory Predicts True and False Memories for Words, Sentences, and Narratives. , 2016, Journal of applied research in memory and cognition.

[30]  N. Kriegeskorte,et al.  Representational geometry: integrating cognition, computation, and the brain , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  D. Schacter,et al.  Can medial temporal lobe regions distinguish true from false? An event-related functional MRI study of veridical and illusory recognition memory , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Marian Berryhill,et al.  True memory, false memory, and subjective recollection deficits after focal parietal lobe lesions. , 2010, Neuropsychology.

[33]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Differential contributions of prefrontal, medial temporal, and sensory-perceptual regions to true and false memory formation. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Yong He,et al.  BrainNet Viewer: A Network Visualization Tool for Human Brain Connectomics , 2013, PloS one.

[35]  K. McDermott,et al.  Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. , 1995 .

[36]  L. Tyler,et al.  Representational Similarity Analysis Reveals Commonalities and Differences in the Semantic Processing of Words and Objects , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  S. Tonegawa,et al.  Creating a False Memory in the Hippocampus , 2013, Science.

[38]  Valerie A. Carr,et al.  Global Similarity and Pattern Separation in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe Predict Subsequent Memory , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Scott A. Guerin,et al.  Memory distortion: an adaptive perspective , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[41]  Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Neural mechanisms of semantic interference and false recognition in short-term memory , 2011, NeuroImage.

[42]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  D. Gallo,et al.  False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion , 2010, Memory & cognition.

[44]  Johan Wessberg,et al.  A Monte Carlo method for locally multivariate brain mapping , 2011, NeuroImage.

[45]  Jonathan Winawer,et al.  GLMdenoise: a fast, automated technique for denoising task-based fMRI data , 2013, Front. Neurosci..

[46]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Decoding information in the human hippocampus: A user's guide , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  P. Dupont,et al.  Similarity of fMRI Activity Patterns in Left Perirhinal Cortex Reflects Semantic Similarity between Words , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al.  Unique semantic space in the brain of each beholder predicts perceived similarity , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  E. Hirshman,et al.  True and False Recognition in MINERVA2: Explanations from a Global Matching Perspective , 1998 .

[51]  Yadin Dudai,et al.  Following the Crowd: Brain Substrates of Long-Term Memory Conformity , 2011, Science.