Spectro-temporal factors in two-dimensional human sound localization.

This paper describes the effect of spectro-temporal factors on human sound localization performance in two dimensions (2D). Subjects responded with saccadic eye movements to acoustic stimuli presented in the frontal hemisphere. Both the horizontal (azimuth) and vertical (elevation) stimulus location were varied randomly. Three types of stimuli were used, having different spectro-temporal patterns, but identically shaped broadband averaged power spectra: noise bursts, frequency-modulated tones, and trains of short noise bursts. In all subjects, the elevation components of the saccadic responses varied systematically with the different temporal parameters, whereas the azimuth response components remained equally accurate for all stimulus conditions. The data show that the auditory system does not calculate a final elevation estimate from a long-term (order 100 ms) integration of sensory input. Instead, the results suggest that the auditory system may apply a "multiple-look" strategy in which the final estimate is calculated from consecutive short-term (order few ms) estimates. These findings are incorporated in a conceptual model that accounts for the data and proposes a scheme for the temporal processing of spectral sensory information into a dynamic estimate of sound elevation.

[1]  N. Viemeister,et al.  Temporal integration and multiple looks. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  Robert A. Butler,et al.  Binaural localization: Influence of stimulus frequency and the linkage to covert peak areas , 1993, Hearing Research.

[3]  Manfred R. Schroeder,et al.  Synthesis of low-peak-factor signals and binary sequences with low autocorrelation (Corresp.) , 1970, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[4]  A D Musicant,et al.  The influence of pinnae-based spectral cues on sound localization. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  Simon R. Oldfield,et al.  Acuity of Sound Localisation: A Topography of Auditory Space. I. Normal Hearing Conditions , 1984, Perception.

[6]  D. W. Batteau,et al.  The role of the pinna in human localization , 1967, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[7]  Paul M. Hofman,et al.  Identification of Spectral Features as Sound Localization Cues in the External Ear Acoustics , 1997, IWANN.

[8]  H. L. Han Measuring a Dummy Head in Search of Pinna Cues , 1994 .

[9]  E. Shaw Transformation of sound pressure level from the free field to the eardrum in the horizontal plane. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  R Meddis,et al.  A physical model of sound diffraction and reflections in the human concha. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  D. M. Green,et al.  Directional sensitivity of sound-pressure levels in the human ear canal. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  C. Harris,et al.  Does saccadic undershoot minimize saccadic flight-time? A Monte-Carlo study , 1995, Vision Research.

[13]  E. Shaw,et al.  External-ear acoustic models with simple geometry. , 1968, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  J. Blauert Spatial Hearing: The Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization , 1983 .

[15]  F. Wightman,et al.  The dominant role of low-frequency interaural time differences in sound localization. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  J. C. Middlebrooks Narrow-band sound localization related to external ear acoustics. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  Simon R. Oldfield,et al.  Acuity of Sound Localisation: A Topography of Auditory Space. II. Pinna Cues Absent , 1984, Perception.

[18]  J. Hebrank,et al.  Spectral cues used in the localization of sound sources on the median plane. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  V. Mellert,et al.  Transformation characteristics of the external human ear. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  F. Wightman,et al.  A model of head-related transfer functions based on principal components analysis and minimum-phase reconstruction. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  M. Cynader,et al.  A computational theory of spectral cue localization , 1993 .

[23]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. I: Stimulus synthesis. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  Mark B. Gardner,et al.  Problem of Localization in the Median Plane , 1972 .

[25]  E D Young,et al.  Neural network models of sound localization based on directional filtering by the pinna. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  J. V. Gisbergen,et al.  Scatter in the metrics of saccades and properties of the collicular motor map , 1989, Vision Research.

[27]  M. Gardner,et al.  Problem of localization in the median plane: effect of pinnae cavity occlusion. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[29]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Precise recording of human eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.