The differential effects of intrinsic and identified motivation on well-being and performance: prospective, experimental, and implicit approaches to self-determination theory.

Self-determination theory research has demonstrated that intrinsic and identified self-regulations are associated with successful adaptation. However, few distinctions are typically made between these regulations and their outcomes. In the present studies, the associations between intrinsic and identified motivations and outcomes of psychological well-being and academic performance are compared in educational settings. In Study 1, intrinsic self-regulation predicted psychological well-being, independent of academic performance. In contrast, identified regulation predicted academic performance. Additionally, the more that students demonstrated an identified academic regulation, the more that their psychological well-being was contingent on performance. In Study 2a, priming intrinsic self-regulation led to greater psychological well-being 10 days later. In Study 2b, an implicit measure of identified regulation predicted academic performance 6 weeks later. Results indicate the need to address important distinctions between intrinsic and identified regulations.

[1]  Kennon M. Sheldon,et al.  Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: the self-concordance model. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  R. Ryan,et al.  Autonomy in children's learning: an experimental and individual difference investigation. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  E. Deci,et al.  Self‐determination theory and work motivation , 2005 .

[4]  G. Maruyama,et al.  The "likeableness" of 555 personality trait words: Ten years later , 1976 .

[5]  Robert J. Vallerand,et al.  Identified and introjected forms of political internalization: Extending self-determination theory. , 1996 .

[6]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Thomas E. Joiner,et al.  A measure of positive and negative affect for children: Scale development and preliminary validation. , 1999 .

[8]  M. Clark,et al.  Research Methods in Personality and Social Psychology , 1990 .

[9]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. , 2000 .

[10]  E. Deci,et al.  The "What" and "Why" of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior , 2000 .

[11]  R. Vallerand,et al.  Self-determination and persistence in a real-life setting: toward a motivational model of high school dropout. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  E. Deci,et al.  Handbook of Self-Determination Research , 2002 .

[13]  Kennon M. Sheldon,et al.  Daily Well-Being: The Role of Autonomy, Competence, and Relatedness , 2000 .

[14]  Kennon M. Sheldon,et al.  Pursuing Personal Goals: Skills Enable Progress, but Not all Progress is Beneficial , 1998 .

[15]  Edward L. Deci,et al.  Inner resources for school achievement : motivational mediators of children's perceptions of their parents , 1991 .

[16]  R. Ryan,et al.  Perceived locus of causality and internalization: examining reasons for acting in two domains. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Kennon M. Sheldon,et al.  Personal goals and psychological growth: testing an intervention to enhance goal attainment and personality integration. , 2002, Journal of personality.

[18]  R. Ryan,et al.  On energy, personality, and health: subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. , 1997, Journal of personality.

[19]  R. Vallerand,et al.  On the cued activation of situational motivation , 2005 .

[20]  E. Deci,et al.  Revitalization through Self-Regulation: The Effects of Autonomous and Controlled Motivation on Happiness and Vitality , 1999 .

[21]  M. Csíkszentmihályi,et al.  Motivation and academic achievement: the effects of personality traits and the quality of experience. , 1991, Journal of personality.

[22]  E. Deci,et al.  The effects of instructors' autonomy support and students' autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-determination theory perspective , 2000 .

[23]  M. Lepper,et al.  Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. , 1996 .

[24]  David C. McClelland,et al.  How Do Self-Attributed and Implicit Motives Differ?. , 1989 .

[25]  E. Deci,et al.  On assimilating identities to the self: A self-determination theory perspective on internalization and integrity within cultures. , 2003 .

[26]  M. Seligman,et al.  Beyond Money , 2004, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[27]  J. T. Austin,et al.  Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. , 1996 .

[28]  Marianne Miserandino,et al.  Children Who Do Well in School: Individual Differences in Perceived Competence and Autonomy in Above-Average Children , 1996 .

[29]  J. R. Salancik Inference of One's Attitude from Behavior Recalled under Linguistically Manipulated Cognitive Sets. , 1974 .

[30]  C. Dweck Self-theories and goals: their role in motivation, personality, and development. , 1990, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[31]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. , 1990 .

[32]  R. Larsen,et al.  The Satisfaction with Life Scale , 1985, Journal of personality assessment.

[33]  C. Dweck Self-Theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personality, and Development. Essays in Social Psychology. , 1999 .

[34]  L. Pelletier,et al.  Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology on the Investigation of Primed and Chronic Autonomous and Heteronomous Motivational Orientations on Behalf Of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology , 2022 .

[35]  T. Chartrand,et al.  THE UNBEARABLE AUTOMATICITY OF BEING , 1999 .

[36]  N. Anderson Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  Kennon M. Sheldon,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Motivating Learning, Performance, and Persistence: The Synergistic Effects of Intrinsic Goal Contents and Autonomy-Supportive Contexts , 2004 .

[38]  R. Vallerand,et al.  Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as predictors of behavior: A prospective study. , 1992 .

[39]  Mark R. Leary,et al.  Handbook of self and identity , 2003 .

[40]  M. Blais,et al.  Significance, structure, and gender differences in life domains of college students , 1990 .

[41]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  M. Zanna,et al.  Direct Experience And Attitude-Behavior Consistency , 1981 .

[43]  R. Ryan,et al.  Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. , 1995, Journal of personality.

[44]  R. Koestner,et al.  Distinguishing three ways of being highly motivated: A closer look at introjection, identification, and intrinsic motivation. , 2002 .

[45]  K. Sheldon,et al.  Coherence and congruence: two aspects of personality integration. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  Mark P. Zanna,et al.  Commitment in the face of adversity : a value-affirmation approach , 1990 .

[47]  M. Mikulincer,et al.  Stress and accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: exploring the normative and intraindividual components of attachment theory. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.