Covert Painting Simulations Influence Aesthetic Appreciation of Artworks

We predicted that performing movements during art perception that resemble the movements of the artist when creating the artwork would enhance aesthetic appreciation. We used painting style as a subtle manipulation of the artists’ original movements during art creation, while matching depicted content and art period across styles. Participants evaluated neoimpressionistic paintings featuring either a hatching-style or a pointillism-style while performing either tipping or hatching-like hand movements. Although participants did not see their own hand movements and were unaware of the link between painting style and hand movements, tipping participants preferred pointillism-style over hatching-style, while hatching participants preferred hatching-style over pointillism-style. Thus, painting style as a static materialization of the painter’s movements can link the original act of art creation with the moment of art perception.

[1]  G. Currie Empathy for Objects1 , 2011 .

[2]  Sascha Topolinski,et al.  Moving the Eye of the Beholder , 2010, Psychological science.

[3]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Embodied Preference Judgments , 2007, Psychological science.

[4]  Sascha Topolinski The sensorimotor contributions to implicit memory, familiarity, and recollection. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  V. Gallese,et al.  Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  P. Haggard,et al.  Extrastriate body area underlies aesthetic evaluation of body stimuli , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  F. Strack,et al.  Motormouth: mere exposure depends on stimulus-specific motor simulations. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  Scott S. Wiltermuth,et al.  Synchrony and Cooperation , 2009, Psychological science.

[9]  Phillip J. Grimaldi,et al.  Uncovering the connection between artist and audience: Viewing painted brushstrokes evokes corresponding action representations in the observer , 2012, Cognition.

[10]  M. Farah,et al.  The psychological reality of the body schema: a test with normal participants. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  N. Schwarz,et al.  Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience? , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[12]  Vernon Lee The Beautiful: An Introduction to Psychological Aesthetics , 2008 .

[13]  Francesco Pavani,et al.  Synchronous Multisensory Stimulation Blurs Self-Other Boundaries , 2010, Psychological science.

[14]  Edward Skidelsky,et al.  The Art Instinct , 2010 .

[15]  F. Strack,et al.  False fame prevented: avoiding fluency effects without judgmental correction. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  H. Leder,et al.  A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. , 2004, British journal of psychology.

[18]  Gerald C. Cupchik,et al.  From perception to production: A multilevel analysis of the aesthetic process. , 1992 .

[19]  Thomas H. Leahey,et al.  Emerging Visions of the Aesthetic Process: Psychology, Semiology, and Philosophy. , 1994 .

[20]  Theodor Lipps Ästhetik : Psychologie des Schönen und der Kunst , 1904 .