The development of long-term lexical representations through Hebb repetition learning

This study clarifies the involvement of short- and long-term memory in novel word-form learning, using the Hebb repetition paradigm. In Experiment 1, participants recalled sequences of visually presented syllables (e.g., la-va-bu-sa-fa-ra-re-si-di), with one particular (Hebb) sequence repeated on every third trial. Crucially, these Hebb sequences contained three orthographic nonword neighbors of existing Dutch base-words (e.g., lavabu – lavabo [kitchen sink]). Twenty-four hours later, the same participants performed two auditory lexicalization tests involving the actual Dutch base-words (e.g., lavabo, safari, residu). Both tests yielded slower reaction times for these Dutch base-words compared with matched control words, which reflects lexical competition between the base-words and the Hebb sequences, therefore demonstrating lexical engagement of the Hebb sequences. In Experiment 2, we subsequently used the Hebb paradigm as an analogue of word-form learning, in order to investigate whether the creation of novel lexical memories requires sleep. Whereas earlier findings indicate that overnight sleep plays a crucial role in lexical consolidation, the current results show that Hebb learning of phonological sequences creates novel word-forms representations in the mental lexicon by the mere passage of time, with sleep playing no necessary role.

[1]  Régine Kolinsky,et al.  The metamorphosis of the statistical segmentation output: Lexicalization during artificial language learning , 2009, Cognition.

[2]  E. Newport,et al.  WORD SEGMENTATION : THE ROLE OF DISTRIBUTIONAL CUES , 1996 .

[3]  Wouter Duyck,et al.  Short Article: The Hebb Repetition Effect as a Laboratory Analogue of Novel Word Learning , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  S. Majerus,et al.  Serial-order short-term memory predicts vocabulary development: evidence from a longitudinal study. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[5]  Christopher M. Conway,et al.  Neurocognitive Basis of Implicit Learning of Sequential Structure and Its Relation to Language Processing , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[6]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  A model for evidence accumulation in the lexical decision task , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  M. Gareth Gaskell,et al.  Overnight lexical consolidation revealed by speech segmentation , 2012, Cognition.

[8]  D. Norris,et al.  The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.

[9]  K. M. Dallenbach,et al.  Obliviscence During Sleep and Waking. , 1924 .

[10]  M. Gareth Gaskell,et al.  Do words go to sleep? Exploring consolidation of spoken forms through direct and indirect measures , 2005 .

[11]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[12]  I. Wilhelm,et al.  The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. , 2009, Sleep medicine reviews.

[13]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  Interfering neighbours: The impact of novel word learning on the identification of visually similar words , 2005, Cognition.

[14]  D. Hebb Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal , 1961 .

[15]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Control of interference during working memory updating. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  G. Hitch,et al.  Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[17]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.12 December 2002 , 2022 .

[18]  M P A Page,et al.  A model linking immediate serial recall, the Hebb repetition effect and the learning of phonological word forms , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  M. Gaskell,et al.  Lexical competition and the acquisition of novel words , 2003, Cognition.

[20]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Exploring the relationship between new word learning and short-term memory for serial order recall, item recall, and item recognition , 2006 .

[21]  Prahlad Gupta,et al.  Examining the Relationship between word Learning, Nonword Repetition, and Immediate Serial Recall in Adults , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[22]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Is there a common mechanism underlying word-form learning and the Hebb repetition effect? : Experimental data and a modelling framework , 2009 .

[23]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[24]  M. Gaskell,et al.  Sleep-Associated Changes in the Mental Representation of Spoken Words , 2007, Psychological science.

[25]  S. Mattys,et al.  Lexical activity in speech processing: Evidence from pause detection , 2002 .

[26]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Relations between vocabulary development and verbal short-term memory: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and item information. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[27]  S. Goldinger Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. , 1998, Psychological review.

[28]  Wouter Duyck,et al.  Order or disorder? Impaired Hebb learning in dyslexia. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  Robert Stickgold,et al.  Sleep Spindle Activity is Associated with the Integration of New Memories and Existing Knowledge , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  A. Baddeley The phonological loop , 2007 .

[31]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Sequential learning in non-human primates , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Lexical learning in bilingual adults: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge , 2008, Cognition.

[33]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Repetition learning in the immediate serial recall of visual and auditory materials. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  P. Gooding,et al.  A meta-analysis of indirect memory tests for novel material in organic amnesics , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Catastrophic Interference in Connectionist Networks: The Sequential Learning Problem , 1989 .

[36]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[37]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Learning and Consolidation of Novel Spoken Words , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  Christopher Jarrold,et al.  Short Article: Hebb Learning, Verbal Short-Term Memory, and the Acquisition of Phonological forms in Children , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[39]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.

[40]  W D Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Differentiating lexical form, meaning, and structure in the neural language system. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  P. Perruchet,et al.  Implicit learning and statistical learning: one phenomenon, two approaches , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  N. Burgess,et al.  A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences , 2006 .

[43]  A. Samuel,et al.  Lexical configuration and lexical engagement: When adults learn new words , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  Prahlad Gupta,et al.  A computational model of nonword repetition , immediate serial recall , and nonword learning , 2008 .

[45]  Wouter Duyck,et al.  Verbal working memory is involved in associative word learning unless visual codes are available , 2003 .