Understanding definitions of minimally verbal across instruments: evidence for subgroups within minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder.

BACKGROUND Minimally verbal (MV) children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are often assumed to be profoundly cognitively impaired and excluded from analyses due to challenges completing standardized testing protocols. A literature aimed at increasing understanding of this subgroup is emerging; however, the many methods used to define MV status make it difficult to compare studies. Understanding how different instruments and definitions used to identify MV children affect sample composition is critical to advance research on this understudied clinical population. METHOD The MV status of 1,470 school-aged children was defined using five instruments commonly used in ASD research. MV sample composition was compared across instruments. Analyses examined the proportion of overlap across MV subgroups and the extent to which child characteristics varied across MV subgroups defined using different definitions or combinations of measures. RESULTS A total of 257 children were classified as MV on at least one instrument. Proportion of overlap between definitions ranged from 3% to 100%. The stringency of definition (i.e. few-to-no vs. some words) was associated with differences in cognitive and adaptive functioning; more stringent definitions yielded greater consistency of MV status across instruments. Cognitive abilities ranged from profoundly impaired to average intelligence; 16% had NVIQ ≥ 70. Approximately half exhibited verbal skills commensurate with nonverbal cognitive ability, whereas half had verbal abilities significantly lower than their estimated NVIQ. CONCLUSIONS Future studies of MV children must carefully consider the methods used to identify their sample, acknowledging that definitions including children with 'some words' may yield larger samples with a wider range of language and cognitive abilities. Broadly defined MV samples may be particularly important to delineate factors interfering with language development in the subgroup of children whose expressive impairments are considerably below their estimated nonverbal cognitive abilities.

[1]  Andrew Pickles,et al.  Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development of language: a 17-year follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. , 2014, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[2]  M. Gernsbacher,et al.  Infant and toddler oral- and manual-motor skills predict later speech fluency in autism. , 2008, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[3]  Pamela Mathy,et al.  Predictors of Phrase and Fluent Speech in Children With Autism and Severe Language Delay , 2013, Pediatrics.

[4]  Catherine Lord,et al.  Standardizing ADOS Domain Scores: Separating Severity of Social Affect and Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors , 2014, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[5]  Christopher Gillberg,et al.  Children with autism spectrum disorders who do not develop phrase speech in the preschool years , 2015, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[6]  M. Rutter,et al.  Cognitive and language skills in adults with autism: a 40-year follow-up. , 2014, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[7]  Helen Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Language Assessment and Development in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[8]  C. Lord,et al.  Assessing the Minimally Verbal School‐Aged Child With Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2013, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[9]  Somer Bishop,et al.  Factors influencing scores on the social responsiveness scale. , 2013, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[10]  N. Brady,et al.  Eye Tracking as a Measure of Receptive Vocabulary in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2014, Augmentative and alternative communication.

[11]  L. Naigles,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language: Language in children with autism spectrum disorders , 2015 .

[12]  D. Almirall,et al.  Communication interventions for minimally verbal children with autism: a sequential multiple assignment randomized trial. , 2014, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[13]  P. Decouflé,et al.  Prevalence of autism in a United States population: the Brick Township, New Jersey, investigation. , 2001, Pediatrics.

[14]  Laurent Mottron,et al.  The Level and Nature of Autistic Intelligence , 2007, Psychological science.

[15]  C. Lord,et al.  The Simons Simplex Collection: A Resource for Identification of Autism Genetic Risk Factors , 2010, Neuron.

[16]  C. Kasari,et al.  Parents’ Adoption of Social Communication Intervention Strategies: Families Including Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Who are Minimally Verbal , 2015, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[17]  C. Lord,et al.  Replication of Standardized ADOS Domain Scores in the Simons Simplex Collection , 2015, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[18]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Comparing methods for assessing receptive language skills in minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders , 2016, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[19]  Paul J. Yoder,et al.  Predicting Spoken Language Level in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2001, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[20]  Connie Kasari,et al.  Minimally Verbal School‐Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Neglected End of the Spectrum , 2013, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[21]  M. Dawson,et al.  Autistic children at risk of being underestimated: school-based pilot study of a strength-informed assessment , 2015, Molecular Autism.