Prediction errors in self- and externally-generated deviants
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Schröger,et al. Selective suppression of self-initiated sounds in an auditory stream: An ERP study. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[2] O. Creutzfeldt,et al. Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe , 1989, Experimental Brain Research.
[3] D. Wolpert,et al. Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[4] Pamela Baess,et al. Action–Sound Coincidences Suppress Evoked Responses of the Human Auditory Cortex in EEG and MEG , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[5] P. Ullsperger,et al. Event‐related potentials in a self‐paced novelty oddball task , 2000, Neuroreport.
[6] Judith M Ford,et al. When it's time for a change: failures to track context in schizophrenia. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[7] Judith M Ford,et al. Dissecting corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[8] K. Lange. The reduced N1 to self-generated tones: an effect of temporal predictability? , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[9] Michael I. Jordan,et al. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.
[10] C. Frith,et al. How do we predict the consequences of our actions? a functional imaging study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[11] Judith M. Ford,et al. The Corollary Discharge in Humans Is Related to Synchronous Neural Oscillations , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] W. Roth,et al. P300—An Orienting Reaction in the Human Auditory Evoked Response , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.
[13] I. Winkler,et al. Mismatch negativity is unaffected by top-down predictive information , 2001, Neuroreport.
[14] David Silbersweig,et al. Functional neuroanatomy of verbal self-monitoring , 1996 .
[15] V. Jousmäki,et al. Attenuation of somatosensory responses to self-produced tactile stimulation. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[16] Paul F. Sowman,et al. Self-initiation and temporal cueing of monaural tones reduce the auditory N1 and P2 , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] I. Winkler,et al. Involuntary Attention and Distractibility as Evaluated with Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2000, Audiology and Neurotology.
[18] Judith M Ford,et al. Anticipating the future: automatic prediction failures in schizophrenia. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[19] Sonja A. Kotz,et al. The Cerebellum Generates Motor-to-Auditory Predictions: ERP Lesion Evidence , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] H. Nittono. Voluntary stimulus production enhances deviance processing in the brain. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[21] F. Panzica,et al. Event-related potential (ERP) markers of melodic processing: The N2 component is modulated by structural complexity, not by melodic ‘meaningfulness’ , 2010, Brain Research Bulletin.
[22] R. Näätänen,et al. Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials , 1982, Biological Psychology.
[23] R. Näätänen. The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[24] J. Ford,et al. Fine-tuning of auditory cortex during speech production. , 2005, Psychophysiology.
[25] R. Hari,et al. Suppressed responses to self-triggered sounds in the human auditory cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.
[26] N. Squires,et al. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[27] P. McGuire,et al. An fMRI study of verbal self-monitoring: neural correlates of auditory verbal feedback. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[28] H. Fukuyama,et al. Cortical processing mechanism for vocalization with auditory verbal feedback , 1997, Neuroreport.
[29] W. Roth,et al. Automatic elicitation of cognitive components by startling stimuli. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[30] J. Ford,et al. Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.
[31] Albert R De Chicchis,et al. Electrophysiologic correlates of attention versus distraction in young and elderly listeners. , 2002, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.
[32] Internal models in human motor control: A computational and psychophysical perspective , 1997 .
[33] L. Schiebinger,et al. Commentary on Risto Naatanen (1990). The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive fenctiono BBS 13s201-2888 , 1991 .
[34] E. Schröger,et al. Early electrophysiological indicators for predictive processing in audition: a review. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[35] D. Linden. The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and What Does It Tell Us? , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[36] Hanjun Liu,et al. Time-dependent Neural Processing of Auditory Feedback during Voice Pitch Error Detection , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] Hanjun Liu,et al. Effect of temporal predictability on the neural processing of self-triggered auditory stimulation during vocalization , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.
[38] K. Crowley,et al. A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[39] Jonathan R. Folstein,et al. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[40] K. Sakai,et al. Brain activations during conscious self‐monitoring of speech production with delayed auditory feedback: An fMRI study , 2003, Human brain mapping.
[41] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[42] E. Schröger,et al. Suppression of the auditory N1 event-related potential component with unpredictable self-initiated tones: evidence for internal forward models with dynamic stimulation. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[43] S. Hillyard,et al. Long-latency evoked potentials to irrelevant, deviant stimuli. , 1976, Behavioral biology.
[44] C. Larson,et al. Error-dependent modulation of speech-induced auditory suppression for pitch-shifted voice feedback , 2011, BMC Neuroscience.
[45] R J Wyatt,et al. Clinical effects of L-5-hydroxytryptophan administration in chronic schizophrenic patients. , 1979, Biological psychiatry.
[46] J. Polich,et al. Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[47] E. Schröger,et al. Specific or general? The nature of attention set changes triggered by distracting auditory events , 2008, Brain Research.
[48] D. Wolpert,et al. Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[49] E. Schafer,et al. Self-Stimulation Alters Human Sensory Brain Responses , 1973, Science.
[50] K. Reinikainen,et al. Event-related potentials to repetition and change of auditory stimuli. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[51] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.