Gustatory, Olfactory and Trigeminal Interactions in a Model Carbonated Beverage

Flavour results from the integration of aroma, taste and chemosensory information within the brain. Understanding interactions between the anatomically separate systems which relay this information is crucial to our appreciation of how different aspects of food contribute to flavour perception and the formulation of successful products. To examine these interactions, a beverage system was created containing elements capable of stimulating gustatory, olfactory and trigeminal systems. Using ingredients relevant to commercial beverages, water, aroma volatiles, tastants and carbonation, enabled examination of the effects of tastant–aroma–carbonation interactions on sensory perception. Samples, selected according to D-optimal designs, were evaluated by a trained panel. Predictive polynomial models were generated from mean panel data to explain variations in the attributes as a function of design factors. Increasing both sugars and acids resulted in increases in perceived flavour. Evidence of differential flavour enhancement profiles of two sugars at perceptually equi-sweet levels, glucose and fructose, supported previous findings in a non-carbonated system (Hewson et al., Food Qual Prefer 19:323–334, 2008). This difference was also evident in models generated for some mouthfeel attributes (tingling and irritant attributes). Carbonation increased sourness, in agreement with previous literature (McLellan et al., J Food Sci 49:1595–1597, 1984; Yau and McDaniel, J Food Sci 57:1412–1416, 1992), but also suppressed sweetness. Interestingly, evaluation of beverages revealed the perception of a bitter aftertaste, which was primarily driven by CO2 level, enhanced by citric acid, and suppressed by increasing sugar concentration. This study provides a comprehensive assessment of the sensory profile of a model carbonated beverage. Analysis provided novel evidence of the influence of multimodal interactions from gustatory, olfactory and trigeminal origin on sensory perception and highlighted the differential effects of two monosaccharides on key sensory attributes.

[1]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Carbonation Interactions with Sweetness and Sourness , 1992 .

[2]  R. McGorrin,et al.  Flavor-Food Interactions , 1996 .

[3]  B. Green Chemesthesis : Pungency as a component of flavor , 1996 .

[4]  R. Pangborn Taste Interrelationships. II. Suprathreshold Solutions of Sucrose and Citric Acid , 1961 .

[5]  A. Calviño,et al.  Interactions between Co2 Oral Pungency and Taste , 1987, Perception.

[6]  M. O'Mahony,et al.  Psychophysical and neurobiological evidence that the oral sensation elicited by carbonated water is of chemogenic origin. , 2000, Chemical senses.

[7]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Sensory evaluation of sweetened flavored carbonated milk beverages , 1989 .

[8]  A. Noble Taste-aroma interactions , 1996 .

[9]  H. Schifferstein,et al.  The effectiveness of different sweeteners in suppressing citric acid sourness , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  B. Green,et al.  Chemesthesis and taste: Evidence of independent processing of sensation intensity , 2005, Physiology & Behavior.

[11]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Suppression of sourness: A comparative study involving mixtures of organic acids and sugars , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  S. Kallithraka,et al.  Effect of pH on Astringency in Model Solutions and Wines , 1997 .

[13]  R. Stevenson,et al.  Pungency in food perception and preference , 1995 .

[14]  H. Bilgen,et al.  Comparison of oral glucose and sucrose solutions on pain response in neonates. , 2000, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[15]  Tracey Hollowood,et al.  Taste–aroma interactions in a citrus flavoured model beverage system: Similarities and differences between acid and sugar type , 2008 .

[16]  M. O'Mahony,et al.  It hurts so good: Oral irritation by spices and carbonated drinks and the underlying neural mechanisms , 2002 .

[17]  Sweetness intensity in low-carbonated beverages. , 2001, Biomolecular engineering.

[18]  J. Prescott,et al.  Responses of PROP taster groups to variations in sensory qualities within foods and beverages , 2004, Physiology & Behavior.

[19]  F. Shann Suckling and sugar reduce pain in babies , 2007, The Lancet.

[20]  T. Hummel,et al.  Intranasal chemosensory function of the trigeminal nerve and aspects of its relation to olfaction , 2002, International archives of occupational and environmental health.

[21]  Taste Interactions of Sweet and Bitter Compounds , 1996 .

[22]  S. Schmidt,et al.  Mouthfeel Detection Threshold and Instrumental Viscosity of Sucrose and High Fructose Corn Syrup Solutions , 2006 .

[23]  Robert A. Frank,et al.  Taste–smell interactions are tastant and odorant dependent , 1988 .

[24]  S. Schmidt,et al.  Descriptive analysis of cola and lemon/lime carbonated beverages , 2006 .

[25]  C. Graaf,et al.  Sweetness flavour interactions in soft drinks. , 1996 .

[26]  H. Lawless,et al.  Astringency of organic acids is related to pH. , 1996, Chemical senses.

[27]  Wenner-Grenska samfundet,et al.  Olfaction and taste , 1963 .

[28]  A. Holley,et al.  Olfactory receptor cell function is affected by trigeminal nerve activity , 1987, Neuroscience Letters.

[29]  J. Verhagen,et al.  The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: Sensory integration , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[30]  P. Breslin,et al.  An overview of binary taste–taste interactions , 2003 .

[31]  L. M. Kennedy,et al.  Identification and Characterization of Human Fructose or Glucose Taste Variants with Hypogeusia for One Monosaccharide but Not for the Other a , 1998, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[32]  M. Levene,et al.  “Sucrose analgesia”: absorptive mechanism or taste perception? , 1999, Archives of disease in childhood. Fetal and neonatal edition.

[33]  M. McDaniel,et al.  The Effect of Carbonation Level on the Sensory Properties of Flavored Milk Beverages , 1991 .

[34]  J. Delwiche The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavor , 2004 .

[35]  B. Green,et al.  Sensory irritation and taste produced by NaCl and citric acid: effects of capsaicin desensitization , 1993 .

[36]  M. O'Mahony,et al.  Sensory properties of citric acid: psychophysical evidence for sensitization, self-desensitization, cross-desensitization and cross-stimulus-induced recovery following capsaicin. , 2000, Chemical senses.

[37]  G. Kracke,et al.  Sugar Solution Analgesia: The Effects of Glucose on Expressed Mu Opioid Receptors , 2005, Anesthesia and analgesia.

[38]  B. M. King,et al.  Sweetener/sweetness-induced changes in flavor perception and flavor release of fruity and green character in beverages. , 2006, Journal of agricultural and food chemistry.

[39]  Harry T. Lawless,et al.  Psychological biases in time-intensity scaling , 1992 .

[40]  B. Cowart The addition of CO2 to traditional taste solutions alters taste quality. , 1998, Chemical senses.

[41]  R. Carbajal,et al.  Randomised trial of analgesic effects of sucrose, glucose, and pacifiers in term neonates , 1999, BMJ.

[42]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Carbonated Water Lexicon: Temperature and CO2 Level Influence on Descriptive Ratings , 1993 .

[43]  M. R. McLELLAN,et al.  Sensory Analysis of Carbonated Apple Juice Using Response Surface Methodology , 1984 .

[44]  G. Brand Olfactory/trigeminal interactions in nasal chemoreception , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[45]  S. Schmidt,et al.  Relationship between physical properties and sensory attributes of carbonated beverages. , 2007, Journal of food science.

[46]  Y. Boucher,et al.  Suppression of Central Taste Transmission by Oral Capsaicin , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  M. O'Mahony,et al.  Neurobiological and Psychophysical Mechanisms Underlying the Oral Sensation Produced by Carbonated Water , 1999, Journal of Neuroscience.