The influence of the indicator arm on end point distribution in proprioceptive localization with multi-joint arms

The present study attempted to demonstrate that the indicator arm influences end point distribution in contralateral multi-joint proprioceptive tasks and also that intrinsic physical characteristics of multi-joint arms (arm stiffness) may predict the error pattern. For this purpose, we carried out two types of contralateral localization tasks with multi-jointed arm movements. In the concurrent localization task, the end point distribution was significantly more elongated in the direction of the lower stiffness at each target position when based on the indicator stiffness, while in the remembered localization task, there was no significant difference between the axes. The best-fit ellipse for the end point distribution also confirmed those results. These findings may support the idea that a large part of the configuration of end point distribution could be determined by the characteristics of arm stiffness of the indicator arm in the condition without memory decay of position representation. Further, error bias of proprioceptive localization may be influenced by the combined effect between movement direction and orientation of the lower stiffness. In conclusion, this study suggests that error patterns largely reflect indicator factors such as the elastic property of the arm in multi-joint proprioceptive tasks, which have been assumed to assess the proprioceptive sense of the reference arm.

[1]  T. Allen,et al.  Position sense at the human forearm in the horizontal plane during loading and vibration of elbow muscles , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  E. Ribot-Ciscar,et al.  Cutaneous afferents provide a neuronal population vector that encodes the orientation of human ankle movements , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  E. Ribot-Ciscar,et al.  Ago-antagonist muscle spindle inputs contribute together to joint movement coding in man , 1998, Brain Research.

[4]  D. F. Collins,et al.  Movement illusions evoked by ensemble cutaneous input from the dorsum of the human hand. , 1996, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  P. Viviani,et al.  Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Proprioceptive Illusions Induced by Muscle Vibration: Contribution by Muscle Spindles to Perception? , 1972, Science.

[7]  D. Burke,et al.  The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration of non‐contracting muscles. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  Daniel J Goble,et al.  Upper limb asymmetries in the perception of proprioceptively determined dynamic position sense. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Christina T Fuentes,et al.  Where is your arm? Variations in proprioception across space and tasks. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Motor commands contribute to human position sense , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  B. Martin,et al.  Position sense asymmetry , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Uwe Proske,et al.  Signals of motor command bias joint position sense in the presence of feedback from proprioceptors. , 2009, Journal of applied physiology.

[13]  D. Ostry,et al.  Learning to control arm stiffness under static conditions. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  J Paillard,et al.  A proprioceptive contribution to the spatial encoding of position cues for ballistic movements. , 1974, Brain research.

[15]  B. Martin,et al.  Age-Related Differences in Upper Limb Proprioceptive Acuity , 2007, Perceptual and motor skills.

[16]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The control of stable postures in the multijoint arm , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Effects of Arm Stiffness and Muscle Effort on Position Reproduction Error in the Horizontal Plane , 2012, Perceptual and motor skills.

[19]  J A Winter,et al.  Muscle spindle signals combine with the sense of effort to indicate limb position , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  Uwe Proske,et al.  Effects of muscle conditioning on position sense at the human forearm during loading or fatigue of elbow flexors and the role of the sense of effort , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  Erin K. Cressman,et al.  Proprioceptive localization of the left and right hands , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Daniel R Lametti,et al.  Control of movement variability and the regulation of limb impedance. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J A Kelso,et al.  Preselection in short-term motor memory. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[24]  Daniel R Lametti,et al.  Postural constraints on movement variability. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Rieko Osu,et al.  Endpoint Stiffness of the Arm Is Directionally Tuned to Instability in the Environment , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  D. Burke,et al.  The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration during isometric contraction. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  Paul L Gribble,et al.  Role of cocontraction in arm movement accuracy. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Afferent input, efference copy, signal noise, and biases in perception of joint angle during active versus passive elbow movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  B. Edin,et al.  Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[31]  Elizabeth T. Wilson,et al.  Mapping Proprioception across a 2D Horizontal Workspace , 2010, PloS one.

[32]  J. S. Scott Kelso,et al.  Planning and Efferent Components in the Coding of Movement , 2013 .

[33]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  D. Goble,et al.  Where was my arm again? Memory-based matching of proprioceptive targets is enhanced by increased target presentation time , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  S C Gandevia,et al.  Effect of eccentric exercise on position sense at the human forearm in different postures. , 2006, Journal of applied physiology.

[36]  J. Roll,et al.  Kinaesthetic role of muscle afferents in man, studied by tendon vibration and microneurography , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  R. Sainburg,et al.  The effect of target modality on visual and proprioceptive contributions to the control of movement distance , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  H. Gomi,et al.  Task-Dependent Viscoelasticity of Human Multijoint Arm and Its Spatial Characteristics for Interaction with Environments , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  S. Scott,et al.  Quantitative Assessment of Limb Position Sense Following Stroke , 2010, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[41]  Y Laufer,et al.  Accuracy of reproducing hand position when using active compared with passive movement. , 2001, Physiotherapy research international : the journal for researchers and clinicians in physical therapy.

[42]  A. Crowe,et al.  Proprioceptive Accuracy in Two Dimensions , 1987, Perceptual and motor skills.