Early Visual Cortex Dynamics during Top–Down Modulated Shifts of Feature-Selective Attention

Shifting attention from one color to another color or from color to another feature dimension such as shape or orientation is imperative when searching for a certain object in a cluttered scene. Most attention models that emphasize feature-based selection implicitly assume that all shifts in feature-selective attention underlie identical temporal dynamics. Here, we recorded time courses of behavioral data and steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs), an objective electrophysiological measure of neural dynamics in early visual cortex to investigate temporal dynamics when participants shifted attention from color or orientation toward color or orientation, respectively. SSVEPs were elicited by four random dot kinematograms that flickered at different frequencies. Each random dot kinematogram was composed of dashes that uniquely combined two features from the dimensions color (red or blue) and orientation (slash or backslash). Participants were cued to attend to one feature (such as color or orientation) and respond to coherent motion targets of the to-be-attended feature. We found that shifts toward color occurred earlier after the shifting cue compared with shifts toward orientation, regardless of the original feature (i.e., color or orientation). This was paralleled in SSVEP amplitude modulations as well as in the time course of behavioral data. Overall, our results suggest different neural dynamics during shifts of attention from color and orientation and the respective shifting destinations, namely, either toward color or toward orientation.

[1]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[2]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Independent effects of attentional gain control and competitive interactions on visual stimulus processing. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  F. Smulders Simplifying jackknifing of ERPs and getting more out of it: retrieving estimates of participants' latencies. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Toward a Unified Theory of Visual Area V4 , 2012, Neuron.

[6]  R. Desimone,et al.  A backward progression of attentional effects in the ventral stream , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8 , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[9]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Toward a Unified Theory of Visual Area V 4 , 2012 .

[11]  M Corbetta,et al.  Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. , 1990, Science.

[12]  Søren K. Andersen,et al.  Effects of Feature-selective and Spatial Attention at Different Stages of Visual Processing , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Modulation of sensory suppression: implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic recording of steadystate visual evoked cortical activity , 2005, Brain Topography.

[15]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[16]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  H J Müller,et al.  Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  R. Shapley,et al.  The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  H. Müller,et al.  Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: Redundancy gains in compound tasks , 2002 .

[20]  E. Nadaraya On Estimating Regression , 1964 .

[21]  S. Zeki,et al.  The temporal order of binding visual attributes , 2006, Vision Research.

[22]  D. Heeger,et al.  Decoding and Reconstructing Color from Responses in Human Visual Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  H. Müller,et al.  Dimension-based visual attention modulates dual-judgment accuracy in Duncan's (1984) one- versus two-object report paradigm. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  José L. Marroquín,et al.  Exploratory EEG data analysis for psychophysiological experiments , 2004, NeuroImage.

[25]  G. Orban,et al.  The organization of orientation selectivity throughout macaque visual cortex. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Klaus Gramann,et al.  Dimension-based attention modulates early visual processing. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[27]  S. Andersen,et al.  Behavioral performance follows the time course of neural facilitation and suppression during cued shifts of feature-selective attention , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  Pierre Jolicœur,et al.  The "red-alert" effect in visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[29]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[30]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  S. Zeki,et al.  A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in area V4 of the macaque: sensitivity to stimulus form. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Receptive field size-dependent attention effects in simultaneously presented stimulus displays , 2006, NeuroImage.

[34]  P. Viviani,et al.  Colour, form, and movement are not perceived simultaneously , 2001, Vision Research.

[35]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[36]  Alex Martin,et al.  Cortical Regions Associated with Perceiving, Naming, and Knowing about Colors , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[38]  H. Müller,et al.  Searching for unknown feature targets on more than one dimension: Investigating a “dimension-weighting” account , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Selective Attention to Task-Irrelevant Emotional Distractors Is Unaffected by the Perceptual Load Associated with a Foreground Task , 2012, PloS one.

[40]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attention to both space and feature modulates neuronal responses in macaque area V4. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.

[42]  Matthias M. Müller Location and features of instructive spatial cues do not influence the time course of covert shifts of visual spatial attention , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[43]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[44]  S. Treue Neural correlates of attention in primate visual cortex , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[45]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Comparison of four methods of heterochromatic photometry. , 1972, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[46]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady‐state visual evoked potential , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[47]  Moshe Gur,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi003 Orientation and Direction Selectivity of Neurons in V1 of Alert Monkeys: Functional Relationships and Laminar Distributions , 2022 .

[48]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  R. B. Reilly,et al.  FASTER: Fully Automated Statistical Thresholding for EEG artifact Rejection , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[50]  H. Müller,et al.  Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  Kazumichi Matsumiya,et al.  Time Courses of Attentional Modulation in Neural Amplification and Synchronization Measured with Steady-state Visual-evoked Potentials , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Attentional Selection of Feature Conjunctions Is Accomplished by Parallel and Independent Selection of Single Features , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  H. Müller,et al.  Visual search and selective attention , 2006 .

[54]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological studies of color processing in human visual cortex. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[55]  S. Zeki A vision of the brain , 1993 .

[56]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[57]  H. Müller,et al.  Locus of dimension weighting: Preattentive or postselective? , 2006 .

[58]  Richard B Buxton,et al.  Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas , 2001, Vision Research.