Understanding of human communicative motives in domestic dogs

Chimpanzees find it easier to locate food when a human prohibits them from going to a certain location than when she indicates that location helpfully. Human children, in contrast, use the cooperative gesture more readily. The question here was whether domestic dogs are more like chimpanzees, in this regard, or more like human children. In our first study we presented 40 dogs with two communicative contexts. In the cooperative context the experimenter informed the subject where food was hidden by pointing helpfully (with a cooperative tone of voice). In the competitive context the experimenter extended her arm towards the correct location in a prohibitive manner, palm of hand out (uttering a forbidding command in a prohibitive tone of voice). Dogs were successful in the cooperative condition (P = 0.005) but chose randomly in the competitive condition (P = 0.221). The second study independently varied the two characteristics of the communicative gesture (the gesture itself and the tone of voice). In addition to replicating dogs’ better performance with the cooperative gestures, this study suggests that tone of voice and context had more effect than type of gesture. In the context of food acquisition, domestic dogs, like human children, seem more prepared to use human gestures when they are given cooperatively.

[1]  S. Shettleworth Fundamentals of Comparative Cognition , 2012 .

[2]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners , 2008, Cognition.

[3]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[4]  Victoria Southgate,et al.  Infant pointing: communication to cooperate or communication to learn? , 2007, Child development.

[5]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The Domestication of Social Cognition in Dogs , 2002, Science.

[6]  S. Gosling,et al.  Personality Dimensions in Nonhuman Animals , 1999 .

[7]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Why Don't Apes Point? , 2006 .

[8]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.

[9]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Bias in interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety. , 1991, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[11]  M. Mendl,et al.  Animal behaviour: Cognitive bias and affective state , 2004, Nature.

[12]  C. Dweck,et al.  A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality , 1988 .

[13]  Márta Gácsi,et al.  The effect of development and individual differences in pointing comprehension of dogs , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[14]  S. Arndt,et al.  A test to identify judgement bias in mice , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[15]  Melissa Bateson,et al.  Larger, enriched cages are associated with 'optimistic' response biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) , 2008 .

[16]  L. M. Dixon Feather Pecking Behaviour and associated Welfare issues in Laying Hens , 2008 .

[17]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. , 1985, Behaviour research and therapy.

[18]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  A comparative approach to dogs’ (Canis familiaris) and human infants’ comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[19]  B. Hare,et al.  Social Cognitive Evolution in Captive Foxes Is a Correlated By-Product of Experimental Domestication , 2005, Current Biology.

[20]  Ludwig Huber,et al.  Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.

[21]  Pearce Animal learning and cognition , 1997 .

[22]  C. Wynne,et al.  Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use , 2000, Animal Cognition.

[24]  Julia Fischer,et al.  Old World Monkeys Compare to Apes in the Primate Cognition Test Battery , 2012, PloS one.

[25]  M. Sutherland,et al.  The effect of method of tail docking on tail-biting behaviour and welfare of pigs , 2009, Animal Welfare.

[26]  H. Selye The evolution of the stress concept. , 1973, American scientist.

[27]  G. Csibra,et al.  Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy , 2006 .

[28]  J. Gomez Species comparative studies and cognitive development , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  M. Tomasello,et al.  12- and 18-Month-Olds Point to Provide Information for Others , 2006 .

[30]  L. Gygax,et al.  Housing induced mood modulates reactions to emotional stimuli in sheep , 2012 .

[31]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Differential Sensitivity to Human Communication in Dogs, Wolves, and Human Infants , 2009, Science.

[32]  M. Mendl,et al.  Disrupting motivational sequences in chicks: Are there affective consequences? , 2013 .

[33]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.

[34]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[35]  D M Weary,et al.  Invited review: The welfare of dairy cattle--key concepts and the role of science. , 2009, Journal of dairy science.

[36]  R. Jones,et al.  Experimental novelty and tonic immobility in chickens ( Gallus domesticus ) , 1984, Behavioural Processes.

[37]  Ádám Miklósi,et al.  Dogs' (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. , 2002 .

[38]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Interpretation of homophones related to threat in anxiety states. , 1989, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[39]  D. Broom,et al.  Animal welfare: concepts and measurement. , 1991, Journal of animal science.

[40]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers , 2004 .

[41]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The domestication hypothesis for dogs' skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008) , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  Richard M A Parker,et al.  Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms , 2009 .

[43]  C. Hewson,et al.  What is animal welfare? Common definitions and their practical consequences. , 2003, The Canadian veterinary journal = La revue veterinaire canadienne.

[44]  M. Tomasello,et al.  How dogs know when communication is intended for them. , 2012, Developmental science.

[45]  V. Csányi,et al.  A nonverbal test of knowledge attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[46]  S. Gosling From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[47]  M. Tomasello,et al.  The early ontogeny of human–dog communication , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[48]  L. Trut Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment , 1999, American Scientist.

[49]  D. Weary,et al.  A Scientific Conception of Animal Welfare that Reflects Ethical Concerns , 1997, Animal Welfare.

[50]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses (Equus caballus) , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[51]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human's perspective , 2009 .

[52]  D. Broom Indicators of poor welfare. , 1986, The British veterinary journal.

[53]  N. Dingemanse,et al.  Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution , 2007, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[54]  F. Mora,et al.  Stress, prefrontal cortex and environmental enrichment: Studies on dopamine and acetylcholine release and working memory performance in rats , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[55]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2001, Journal of comparative psychology.

[56]  A. Boissy,et al.  A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[57]  M. Bateson,et al.  Cognitive bias in the chick anxiety–depression model , 2011, Brain Research.

[58]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs , 1998, Animal Cognition.

[59]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  C. M. Coppens,et al.  Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: Towards understanding the biology of individual variation , 2010, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology.

[61]  J. M. Digman PERSONALITY STRUCTURE: EMERGENCE OF THE FIVE-FACTOR MODEL , 1990 .

[62]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  A comparative analysis of animals' understanding of the human pointing gesture , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[63]  D. Weary,et al.  Pain and Pessimism: Dairy Calves Exhibit Negative Judgement Bias following Hot-Iron Disbudding , 2013, PloS one.

[64]  I. Duncan,et al.  Animal welfare defined in terms of feelings , 1996 .

[65]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[66]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis , 2007, Science.

[67]  M. Mendl,et al.  An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[68]  L. Keeling,et al.  Cognitive bias and anticipatory behaviour of laying hens housed in basic and enriched pens , 2012 .

[69]  M. Meaney,et al.  Environmental Enrichment Reverses the Effects of Maternal Separation on Stress Reactivity , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  Wim Verbeke,et al.  Ethical Challenges for Livestock Production:Meeting Consumer Concerns about Meat Safety and AnimalWelfare , 2000 .

[71]  Gabriella Lakatos,et al.  A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[72]  C. Castellini,et al.  EFFECT OF CONVENTIONAL VERSUS ORGANIC METHOD OF PRODUCTION ON THE BROILER CARCASS AND MEAT QUALITY , 2002 .

[73]  Georgia Mason,et al.  Can't stop, won't stop: is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator? , 2004 .

[74]  S. J. Lachman,et al.  Learning is a Process: Toward an Improved Definition of Learning , 1997 .

[75]  C. W. Ragsdale,et al.  Cell-type homologies and the origins of the neocortex , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[76]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Preferences for fixed and variable food sources: variability in amount and delay. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[77]  M. Bateson,et al.  Stereotyping starlings are more ‘pessimistic’ , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[78]  L. Pedersen,et al.  Aggression, fearfulness and coping styles in female pigs , 2003 .

[79]  J. Lassen,et al.  Happy pigs are dirty! – conflicting perspectives on animal welfare , 2006 .

[80]  S. H. Richter,et al.  A glass full of optimism: Enrichment effects on cognitive bias in a rat model of depression , 2012, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[81]  M. Bertini,et al.  Beneficial effects of enriched environment on adolescent rats from stressed pregnancies , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[82]  Oliver von Bohlen und Halbach,et al.  Ambiguous-Cue Interpretation is Biased Under Stress- and Depression-Like States in Rats , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[83]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  A Simple Reason for a Big Difference Wolves Do Not Look Back at Humans, but Dogs Do , 2003, Current Biology.

[84]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) Use Human and Conspecific Social Cues to Locate Hidden Food , 1999 .

[85]  D. Dewsbury,et al.  Tonic immobility and the dorsal immobility response in twelve species of muroid rodents. , 1981, Behavioral and neural biology.

[86]  Márta Gácsi,et al.  Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[87]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  When dogs seem to lose their nose: an investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner , 2003 .

[88]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Apes' and children's understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation. , 2006, Developmental science.

[89]  R. Geers,et al.  Measuring and Monitoring Animal Welfare: Transparency in the Food Product Quality Chain , 2003, Animal Welfare.

[90]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective , 2004 .

[91]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. , 2004, Developmental science.

[92]  J. Russell Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. , 2003, Psychological review.

[93]  S. Shettleworth Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .

[94]  L. Dugatkin,et al.  Boldness, behavioral inhibition and learning , 2003 .

[95]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Human-like social skills in dogs? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[96]  M. Bateson,et al.  Performance on a categorisation task suggests that removal of environmental enrichment induces ‘pessimism’ in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) , 2007, Animal Welfare.

[97]  Jonathan J. Cooper,et al.  Clever hounds: social cognition in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) , 2003 .

[98]  K. Quigley,et al.  Exploratory behavior in mice selectively bred for developmental differences in aggressive behavior. , 2008, Developmental psychobiology.

[99]  S. D. de Boer,et al.  Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology , 1999, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[100]  M. Bateson,et al.  Agitated Honeybees Exhibit Pessimistic Cognitive Biases , 2011, Current Biology.