Perception of partly occluded objects by young chicks

Completion of partly occluded objects is a ubiquitous phenomenon in human visual perception. It is unclear, however, whether it occurs at all in other species: Studies on visual discrimination learning have revealed that animals usually attend to parts and features of the discriminative stimuli rather than to global object properties. We provide here the first demonstration of recognition of partly occluded objects in a bird species, the domestic chickGallus gallus, using the naturalistic setting made available by filial imprinting, a process whereby young birds form attachments to their mothers or some artificial substitute. In Experiment 1, newborn chicks were reared singly with a red cardboard triangle, to which they rapidly imprinted and therefore treated as a social partner. On Day 3 of life, the chicks were presented with pairs of objects composed of either isolated fragments or occluded parts of the imprinting stimulus. Chicks consistently chose to associate with complete or with partly occluded versions of the imprinting object rather than with separate fragments of it. Similarly, in Experiment 2, chicks reared with a partly occluded triangle chose to associate with a complete triangle rather than with a fragmented one, whereas chicks reared with a fragmented triangle chose to associate with a fragmented triangle and not with a complete one. Newborn chicks thus appear to behave as if they could experience amodal completion.

[1]  G. Horn,et al.  The role of a restricted region of the chick forebrain in the recognition of individual conspecifics , 1987, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  Shigeru Watanabe,et al.  Discrimination of Individuals in Pigeons , 1990 .

[3]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Lateralization of response by chicks to change in a model partner , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  E. Spelke,et al.  Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  R. Andrew The chick in experiment: techniques and tests , 1991 .

[6]  A. Michotte,et al.  Les compléments amodaux des structures perceptives , 1964 .

[7]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affiliation and social discrimination produced by brief exposure in day-old domestic chicks , 1975, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  L. Regolin,et al.  Object and spatial representations in detour problems by chicks , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  Johan J. Bolhuis,et al.  MECHANISMS OF AVIAN IMPRINTING: A REVIEW , 1991, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[10]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Mechanisms of contour perception in monkey visual cortex. I. Lines of pattern discontinuity , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Michotte The perception of causality , 1963 .

[12]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Mechanisms of contour perception in monkey visual cortex. II. Contours bridging gaps , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  G. Horn,et al.  Memory, Imprinting and the Brain , 1985 .

[14]  K. Koffka Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .

[15]  Alan Slater,et al.  Newborn and older infants' perception of partly occluded objects☆ , 1990 .

[16]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Cats see subjective contours , 1988, Vision Research.

[17]  G. Vallortigara Affiliation and aggression as related to gender in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[18]  G. Kanizsa,et al.  Amodal Completion in Mouse Vision , 1993, Perception.

[19]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Differential involvement of right and left hemisphere in individual recognition in the domestic chick , 1994, Behavioural Processes.

[20]  P. Kellman,et al.  A theory of visual interpolation in object perception , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  D. K. Candland,et al.  Discriminability of facial regions used by the domestic chicken in maintaining the social dominance order. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[22]  Towe Al A study of figural equivalence in the pigeon. , 1954 .

[23]  P. Kellman,et al.  Perception of Partly Occluded Objects and Illusory Figures : Evidence for an Identity Hypothesis , 2004 .

[24]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses. , 1984, Science.

[25]  John Cerella,et al.  The pigeon's analysis of pictures , 1980, Pattern Recognit..

[26]  H. Karten,et al.  The Origins of Neocortex: Connections and Lamination as Distinct Events in Evolution , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Right hemisphere advantage for social recognition in the chick , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  R. Bradshaw Discrimination of group members by laying hens Gallus domesticus , 1991, Behavioural Processes.

[29]  K. Lorenz The Companion in the Bird's World , 1937 .

[30]  L. Regolin,et al.  Detour behaviour in the domestic chick: searching for a disappearing prey or a disappearing social partner , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  G. Orban,et al.  Illusory contour orientation discrimination in the cat , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[32]  M. Garrett,et al.  Perceptual knowledge of objects in infancy , 1982 .

[33]  R. Andrew Neural and behavioural plasticity : the use of the domestic chick as a model , 1991 .

[34]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition , 1993 .

[35]  D. Ingle Mechanisms of Shape-Recognition Among Vertebrates , 1978 .

[36]  Sharon L. Greene,et al.  Pigeon visual memory capacity. , 1984 .

[37]  T. Bower Phenomenal identity and form perception in an infant , 1967 .