Visual–vestibular stimulation interferes with information processing in young and older humans

Attention has been implicated in postural control and other tasks requiring sensory integration. The purpose of this study was to investigate the role of attention in sensory-motor processing of vestibular and combined visual–vestibular information during seated rotations using a dual-task interference approach. We hypothesized that auditory information processing would be influenced by concurrent visual-ocular, vestibulo-ocular, or combined visual-vestibulo-ocular processing. We further hypothesized that the effect would be greater in older subjects. Twenty older subjects (10 women, 10 men, 69.3±3.2 years) and 20 young subjects (10 women, 10 men, 23.5±2.9 years) were asked to perform information-processing tasks while they underwent several types of vestibular, visual–vestibular, and ocular motor paradigms. The information-processing tasks were: (1) an auditory simple reaction-time task (SRT), (2) an auditory go–no-go (disjunctive) reaction-time task (DRT), and (3) an auditory forced-choice task (CRT). The visual–vestibular-ocular motor conditions included: (1) no movement/darkness (NO), (2) no movement/fixation (FIX), (3) no movement/pursuit (P), (4) earth-vertical axis rotation (EVAR) in darkness, (5) EVAR with fixation (E-FIX), (6) off-vertical axis rotation (OVAR) in darkness, and (7) OVAR with fixation (O-FIX). Results showed that older subjects had longer reaction times for all combinations of stimulus condition and reaction-time task compared with young subjects. Compared with the NO baseline, reaction times during EVAR were longer for young and older subjects and during OVAR were longer for the young subjects. For FIX and P, the reaction times during P exceeded those during FIX and during NO for both groups. For E-FIX and O-FIX, reaction times did not differ from those during EVAR and OVAR. The interference with information processing by concurrent vestibular stimulation in the dark may be based upon cortical inhibition of auditory processes by vestibular stimulation. Eye movements induced by EVAR showed an increased phase lead during reaction-time tasks, suggesting altered vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) dynamics, possibly based on cerebellar-mediated changes in velocity storage. Since fixation of a head-fixed visual target did not add to the effect of rotation in the dark, a further implication of our results is that VOR-fixation while performing a concurrent information-processing task may be accomplished primarily by VOR suppression rather than by VOR cancellation.

[1]  J. Frank,et al.  Influence of a visuo-spatial, verbal and central executive working memory task on postural control. , 2001, Gait & posture.

[2]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Postural sway characteristics of the elderly under normal and altered visual and support surface conditions. , 1991, Journal of gerontology.

[3]  P. Baltes,et al.  Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[4]  N Lavie,et al.  Effect of articulatory and mental tasks on postural control. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[5]  Michael Ariel,et al.  A “beat-to-beat” interval generator for optokinetic nystagmus , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[6]  D. Zee,et al.  Tilt suppression of vestibulo-ocular reflex in patients with cerebellar lesions. , 1988, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[7]  D. A. Robinson,et al.  A model of quick phase generation in the vestibuloocular reflex , 1978, Biological Cybernetics.

[8]  J M Morse,et al.  Characteristics of the fall-prone patient. , 1987, The Gerontologist.

[9]  Nick Fogt,et al.  The Neurology of Eye Movements, 3rd ed. , 2000 .

[10]  M. Woollacott,et al.  The effects of two types of cognitive tasks on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of falls. , 1997, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[11]  M. Woollacott,et al.  Cognitive influence on postural stability: a neuromuscular analysis in young and older adults. , 2000, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[12]  J. Allum,et al.  Visual and vestibular contributions to pitch sway stabilization in the ankle muscles of normals and patients with bilateral peripheral vestibular deficits , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  J. Demer,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction with telescopic spectacles. , 1991, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[14]  W. E. Collins Effects of mental set upon vestibular nystagmus. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[15]  B. Kerr,et al.  Cognitive spatial processing and the regulation of posture. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  The influence of aging and attentional demands on recovery from postural instability , 1990, Aging.

[17]  I. Donaldson Control of gaze by brain stem neurons Proceedings of the symposium held in the Abbaye de Royaumont. Paris 12–15 July, 1977.Developments in Neuroscience, vol. 1.R. Baker &A. Berthoz (eds). Elsevier/North Holland Biomedical Press, Amsterdam (1977). 514 + xv pp., $59.95 , 1978, Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Goebel,et al.  Interlaboratory Variability of Rotational Chair Test Results , 1994 .

[19]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Age Related Decline in Postural Control Mechanisms , 1989, International journal of aging & human development.

[20]  M. Redfern,et al.  Effect of aging on the otolith-ocular reflex. , 2001, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[21]  Collins We Manipulation of arousal and its effects on human vestibular nystagmus induced by caloric irrigation and angular accelerations. , 1963 .

[22]  W. E. Collins,et al.  Manipulation of arousal and its effects on human vestibular nystagmus induced by caloric irrigation and angular accelerations. , 1963, Aerospace medicine.

[23]  A M Wing,et al.  Age differences in postural stability are increased by additional cognitive demands. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[24]  M. Redfern,et al.  Psychological factors influencing recovery from balance disorders. , 2001, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[25]  T. Brandt,et al.  Reciprocal inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction. Visual motion stimulation deactivates the parieto-insular vestibular cortex. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  G D Paige,et al.  Senescence of human visual-vestibular interactions. 1. Vestibulo-ocular reflex and adaptive plasticity with aging. , 1992, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[27]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Critical Band Width in Loudness Summation , 1957 .

[28]  C. Wall,et al.  Vestibular function in periodic alternating nystagmus. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  C. Bard,et al.  Posture and elderly persons : evidence for deficits in the central integrative mechanisms , 1992 .

[30]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Attentional Activation of the Cerebellum Independent of Motor Involvement , 1997, Science.

[31]  M. Redfern,et al.  Attention influences sensory integration for postural control in older adults. , 2001, Gait & posture.

[32]  L. Nashner Fixed patterns of rapid postural responses among leg muscles during stance , 1977, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  J. Furman,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction during off-vertical axis rotation. , 1996, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[34]  J. Droulez,et al.  Eye movements induced by off-vertical axis rotation (OVAR) at small angles of tilt , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  M. Woollacott,et al.  Attentional demands and postural recovery: the effects of aging. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[36]  G. Andersson,et al.  A dual-task study of interference between mental activity and control of balance. , 1998, The American journal of otology.

[37]  M. Woollacott,et al.  Attentional demands and postural control: the effect of sensory context. , 2000, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[38]  N Lavie,et al.  Attentional demands of perception of passive self-motion in darkness , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  N Teasdale,et al.  Upright standing and gait: are there changes in attentional requirements related to normal aging? , 1996, Experimental aging research.

[40]  Michael A. Gresty,et al.  CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF THE VESTIBULAR SYSTEM, 3RD EDN , 2002 .

[41]  Changes in the horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex of the rhesus monkey with behavioral and pharmacological alerting , 1981, Brain Research.

[42]  P. Bonding Critical bandwidth in loudness summation in sensorineural hearing loss. , 1979, British journal of audiology.

[43]  J. Furman,et al.  Off-Vertical Axis Rotation: A Test of the Otolith-Ocular Reflex , 1992, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[44]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[45]  N Teasdale,et al.  On the cognitive penetrability of posture control. , 1993, Experimental aging research.

[46]  R Platt,et al.  Serious falls in hospitalized patients: correlates and resource utilization. , 1995, The American journal of medicine.

[47]  J. Janken,et al.  Patient Falls in the Acute Care Setting: Identifying Risk Factors , 1986, Nursing research.

[48]  Mark S Redfern,et al.  Attentional dynamics in postural control during perturbations in young and older adults. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[49]  R. Baloh,et al.  The sleep-wake transition in the oculomotor system , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  T. Brandt,et al.  Sensory system interactions during simultaneous vestibular and visual stimulation in PET , 2002, Human brain mapping.