Reproducibility of Distance and Direction Errors Associated with Forward, Backward, and Sideway Walking in the Context of Blind Navigation

The ability to navigate without vision towards a previously seen target has been extensively studied, but its reliability over time has yet to be established. Our aims were to determine distance and direction errors made during blind navigation across four different directions involving three different gait patterns (stepping forward, stepping sideway, and stepping backward), and to establish the test – retest reproducibility of these errors. Twenty young healthy adults participated in two testing sessions separated by 7 days. They were shown targets located, respectively, 8 m ahead, 8 m behind, and 8 m to their right and left. With vision occluded by opaque goggles, they walked forward (target ahead), backward (target behind), and sideway (right and left targets) until they perceived to be on the target. Subjects were not provided with feedback about their performance. Walked distance, angular deviation, and body rotation were measured. The mean estimated distance error was similar across the four walking directions and ranged from 16 to 80 cm with respect to the 8 m target. In contrast, direction errors were significantly larger during sideway navigation (walking in the frontal plane: leftward, 10° ± 15° deviation; rightward, 18° ± 13°) than during forward and backward navigation (walking in the sagittal plane). In general, distance and direction errors were only moderately reproducible between the two sessions [intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) ranging from 0.682 to 0.705]. Among the four directions, rightward navigation showed the best reproducibility with ICCs ranging from 0.607 to 0.726, and backward navigation had the worst reliability with ICCs ranging from 0.094 to 0.554. These findings indicate that errors associated with blind navigation across different walking directions and involving different gait patterns are only moderately to poorly reproducible on repeated testing, especially for walking backward. The biomechanical constraints and increased cognitive loading imposed by changing the walking pattern to backward stepping may underlie the poor performance in this direction.

[1]  M A Goodale,et al.  The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[2]  J M Loomis,et al.  Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  N. Vuillerme,et al.  Veering in human locomotion: modulatory effect of attention , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[4]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  S S Fukusima,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  J. Fung,et al.  Spatial navigation after surgical resection of an acoustic neuroma: pilot study. , 2003, The Journal of otolaryngology.

[8]  E Lindberg,et al.  Acquisition of different types of locational information in cognitive maps: Automatic or effortful processing? , 1983, Psychological research.

[9]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  M Bonanni,et al.  Test-retest reliability of the Fukuda Stepping Test. , 1998, Physiotherapy research international : the journal for researchers and clinicians in physical therapy.

[11]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  J T Corlett,et al.  The effect of perceived locomotor constraints on distance estimation. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  H. Barbeau,et al.  Attentional requirements of walking in spinal cord injured patients compared to normal subjects , 1999, Spinal Cord.

[14]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Differential effects of labyrinthine dysfunction on distance and direction during blindfolded walking of a triangular path , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  The Contributions of Static Visual Cues, Nonvisual Cues, and Optic Flow in Distance Estimation , 2004, Perception.

[16]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992 .

[17]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[18]  永福 智志 The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  T. Gärling,et al.  Maintenance of orientation during locomotion in unfamiliar environments. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of walking speed and prior practice on locomotor distance estimation. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[22]  Jean-Arcady Meyer,et al.  BIOLOGICALLY BASED ARTIFICIAL NAVIGATION SYSTEMS: REVIEW AND PROSPECTS , 1997, Progress in Neurobiology.

[23]  N. Burgess,et al.  Complementary memory systems: competition, cooperation and compensation , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.