Cross-Cultural Differences in the Effects of Abstract and Concrete Thinking on Imagery Generation and Ad Persuasion

ABSTRACT This study examines the effect of culture on imagery generation and ad attitudes. Although research suggests that concrete stimuli generate more images than abstract stimuli, our study shows that this finding is not universal across cultures. Chinese generate more imagery than Americans when encountering abstract stimuli because Chinese tend to think concretely. Moreover, Chinese and Americans have different attitudes toward different stimuli. While Chinese prefer concrete stimuli to abstract stimuli, Americans have the same attitudes toward concrete and abstract stimuli.

[1]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. , 2001, Psychological review.

[2]  J. Richardson Concreteness, imagery, and semantic categorization , 1980 .

[3]  Fred W. Morgan,et al.  Advertising Strategy in China , 2002 .

[4]  H. H. Kassarjian Content Analysis in Consumer Research , 1977 .

[5]  Linda L. Price,et al.  The Role of Imagery in Information Processing: Review and Extensions , 1987 .

[6]  The role of imagery in models of cognition. , 1977 .

[7]  N. Hafer,et al.  Managing cultural differences. , 1986, Nursing management.

[8]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. , 1999 .

[9]  Sharon Shavitt,et al.  Persuasion and culture: Advertising appeals in individualistic and collectivistic societies. , 1994 .

[10]  H. Triandis,et al.  Multimethod probes of individualism and collectivism. , 1990 .

[11]  Barbara Mueller,et al.  Reflections of Culture: An Analysis of Japanese and American Advertising Appeals. , 1986 .

[12]  Sharon Ng,et al.  Exemplars or beliefs? The impact of self-view on the nature and relative influence of brand associations , 2006 .

[13]  J. Edward Kidder,et al.  Japanese Culture: A Study of Origins and Characteristics. , 1974 .

[14]  H. Rao Unnava,et al.  An Imagery-Processing View of the Role of Pictures in Print Advertisements , 1991 .

[15]  Allan Paivio,et al.  Imaginal and verbal mediators and noun concreteness in paired-associate learning: The elusive interaction , 1970 .

[16]  Robert T. Moran Managing Cultural Differences , 1979 .

[17]  H. Markus,et al.  Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. , 1991 .

[18]  Katherine Gallagher,et al.  Advertising and Cultural Values: Reflections in the Distorted Mirror , 1990 .

[19]  B. Hannover,et al.  The semantic--procedural interface model of the self: the role of self-knowledge for context-dependent versus context-independent modes of thinking. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  E. J. Bourne,et al.  Does the Concept of the Person Vary Cross-Culturally? , 1982 .

[21]  John C. Mowen,et al.  The separateness‐connectedness self‐schema: Scale development and application to message construction , 1997 .

[22]  J. Kroll,et al.  Matching words to concepts in two languages: A test of the concept mediation model of bilingual representation , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[23]  G. Chakraborty,et al.  Alternative Modes of Self-Construal: Dimensions of Connectedness–Separateness and Advertising Appeals to the Cultural and Gender-Specific Self , 2000 .

[24]  Hajime Nakamura,et al.  Ways of Thinking of Eastern Peoples: India, China, Tibet, Japan , 1964 .

[25]  A. Paivio Mental imagery in associative learning and memory , 1969 .

[26]  S. Cousins Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. , 1989 .

[27]  M. Cole,et al.  Cognitive Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations , 1976 .

[28]  Peter Wright,et al.  Product Class Advertising Effects on First-Time Buyers' Decision Strategies , 1980 .

[29]  Bruce D. Gelb,et al.  Matching Advertising Appeals to Culture: The Influence of Products' Use Conditions , 1996 .

[30]  A. Paivio,et al.  Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? , 1973 .

[31]  Joan Meyers-Levy,et al.  Evaluating persuasion-enhancing techniques from a resource-matching perspective , 1997 .

[32]  G. Zinkhan,et al.  Cultural Differences and Advertising Expression: A Comparative Content Analysis of Japanese and U.S. Magazine Advertising , 1987 .

[33]  G. Bower Imagery as a relational organizer in associative learning , 1970 .

[34]  Allan K. K. Chan,et al.  A content analysis of connectedness vs. separateness themes used in US and PRC print advertisements , 2001 .

[35]  Montague Ullman The Role of Imagery , 1975 .

[36]  D. C. Phillips Holistic Thought in Social Science , 1976 .

[37]  D. R. Lehman,et al.  What's wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales?: The reference-group effect. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  A. Paivio,et al.  Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[39]  J. Kroll,et al.  Lexical and conceptual memory in the bilingual: mapping form to meaning in two languages , 2020, The Bilingualism Reader.

[40]  Barbara Mueller,et al.  Standardization vs. specialization: An examination of Westernization in Japanese advertising. , 1992 .

[41]  P. Bone,et al.  The Generation and Consequences of Communication-evoked Imagery , 1992 .