Group Morality and Intergroup Relations: Cross-Cultural and Experimental Evidence

An observational, cross-cultural study and an experimental study assessed behaviors indicative of a moral code that condones, and even values, hostility toward outgroups. The cross-cultural study, which used data from the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample (Murdock & White, 1969), found that for preindustrial societies, as loyalty to the ingroup increased the tendency to value outgroup violence more than ingroup violence increased, as did the tendencies to engage in more external than internal warfare, and enjoy war. The experimental study found that relative to guilt-prone group members who were instructed to remain objective, guilt-prone group members who were instructed to be empathic with their ingroup were more competitive in an intergroup interaction. The findings from these studies suggest that group morality is associated with intergroup conflict.

[1]  L. Kohlberg Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization , 1969 .

[2]  K. A. Finlay,et al.  The role of empathy in improving intergroup relations. , 1999 .

[3]  Rick H. Hoyle,et al.  Perceptions of Social Behavior , 1989 .

[4]  A. E. Murphy The Nature and Destiny of Man, Vol. I: Human Nature , 1943 .

[5]  Reinhold Niebuhr,et al.  The Nature and Destiny of Man , 1944 .

[6]  C. Insko,et al.  Memory for and experience of differential competitive behavior of individuals and groups. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  M. Ember On the Origin and Extension of the Incest Taboo , 1975 .

[8]  H. Tajfel,et al.  An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. , 1979 .

[9]  Valerie Wheeler Nammour Drums and guns : a cross-cultural study of the nature of war , 1975 .

[10]  C. Daniel Batson,et al.  Empathy-induced altruism in a prisoner's dilemma , 1999 .

[11]  Michael Cole,et al.  Ethnocentrism: Theories of conflict, ethnic attitudes, and group behavior. , 1975 .

[12]  M. Deutsch,et al.  A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgement. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[13]  Thomas Lickona,et al.  Moral development and behavior : theory, research, and social issues , 1976 .

[14]  E. Cashdan Ethnocentrism and Xenophobia: A Cross‐Cultural Study1 , 2001, Current Anthropology.

[15]  C. Ember,et al.  War, Socialization, and Interpersonal Violence , 1994 .

[16]  H. Lang CONAN - An Electronic Code-Text Data-Base for Cross-Cultural Studies , 1995 .

[17]  Seyuan Shu On the nature and destiny of man , 1933 .

[18]  Lowell Gaertner,et al.  Intragroup social influence and intergroup competition. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  M. Sobel Asymptotic Confidence Intervals for Indirect Effects in Structural Equation Models , 1982 .

[20]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Ronald G. Stansfield,et al.  Sociological Methodology 1982 , 1983 .

[22]  D. Ausubel,et al.  Relationships between shame and guilt in the socializing process. , 1955, Psychological review.

[23]  M. Brewer The Psychology of Prejudice: Ingroup Love and Outgroup Hate? , 1999 .

[24]  W. Hamilton,et al.  The evolution of cooperation. , 1984, Science.

[25]  H. Tajfel,et al.  Social categorization and intergroup behaviour , 1971 .

[26]  C. Insko,et al.  Interindividual-intergroup discontinuity as a function of trust and categorization: the paradox of expected cooperation. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  M. Ridley The origins of virtue : human instincts and the evolution of cooperation , 1998 .

[28]  H. Tajfel Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations , 1982 .

[29]  J. Duckitt,et al.  Group identification and intergroup attitudes: a longitudinal analysis in South Africa. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  C. Insko,et al.  A paradox of individual and group morality: Social psychology as empirical philosophy , 2006 .

[31]  D. R. White,et al.  Standard Cross-Cultural Sample , 1969 .

[32]  J. Victoroff The Mind of the Terrorist , 2005 .

[33]  J. Thibaut,et al.  Procedural Justice: A Psychological Analysis , 1976 .

[34]  Donald T. Campbell,et al.  Ethnocentric and other altruistic motives , 1966 .

[35]  Rick H. Hoyle,et al.  Individual-Group Discontinuity as a Function of Fear and Greed , 1990 .

[36]  C. Insko,et al.  Reduction of interindividual-intergroup discontinuity: the role of leader accountability and proneness to guilt. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  Marc Sageman,et al.  Understanding terror networks. , 2004, International journal of emergency mental health.

[38]  Bharat Maldé,et al.  Interpersonal relations — a theory of interdependence , 1979 .

[39]  J Matthew Webster,et al.  The role of public exposure in moral and nonmoral shame and guilt. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  S. Fiske,et al.  Social Psychology , 2019, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.

[41]  J. Tangney,et al.  Self-relevant emotions. , 2003 .

[42]  M. Ross,et al.  Political decision making and conflict: additional cross-cultural codes and sacales , 1983 .

[43]  David A. Lishner,et al.  “... As you Would have Them Do Unto You”: Does Imagining Yourself in the Other's Place Stimulate Moral Action? , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[44]  A. Bandura,et al.  Mechanisms of Moral Disengagement in the Exercise of Moral Agency , 1996 .

[45]  P. Hoodbhoy Fatal Friendships , 2002 .

[46]  S. Schwartz,et al.  Intergroup aggression: its predictors and distinctness from in-group bias. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  Stuart S. Nagel,et al.  Procedural Justice: A Psychological Analysis , 1976 .

[48]  S. West,et al.  A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[49]  T. Ketelaar,et al.  The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction , 2003, Cognition & emotion.

[50]  T. Ferguson,et al.  Guilt, shame, and symptoms in children. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[51]  Jack L. Vevea,et al.  Beyond the group mind: a quantitative review of the interindividual-intergroup discontinuity effect. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[52]  Brian P. Meier,et al.  A comparison of human aggression committed by groups and individuals: An interindividual–intergroup discontinuity☆ , 2004 .

[53]  C. Daniel Batson,et al.  Empathy-induced altruism in a prisoner's dilemma II: what if the target of empathy has defected? , 2001 .

[54]  Mark R. Leary,et al.  Handbook of self and identity , 2003 .

[55]  R. Sabatelli The Social Psychology of Groups , 2000 .

[56]  G. L. Bon,et al.  Scientific Literature: The Crowd. A Study of the Popular Mind , 1897 .

[57]  H. Bloom,et al.  A SCIENTIFIC EXPEDITION INTO THE FORCES OF HISTORY , 1995 .

[58]  John C. Turner,et al.  Self-categorization theory and social influence. , 1989 .