Cognitive attribution of the source of an error in object-lifting results in differences in motor generalization

To lift an object, the motor system must predict the weight of the object and use this information to program appropriate lifting forces. If this prediction is erroneous, people may assign blame for the error to either themselves or an external source—a process called credit assignment. In the present study, we explored the role of credit assignment on weight predictions during a lifting task. Participants were told that the EMG surface electrodes attached to their lifting hand were either part of a “passive” system that recorded muscular activity, or part of an “active” system that would apply energy to the muscle, influencing weight perception. Participants performed 90 lifts of the training blocks, followed by 10 lifts of a newly encountered larger test block. In between training and test trials, the experimenter turned off the recording system and removed the surface electrodes for participants in the “active” group. For each lift, we determined the initial peak rate of change of vertical load force rate and load-phase duration, estimates of predicted object weight. Analysis of the first 10 training lifts and the last 10 training lifts revealed no effect of Active versus Passive EMG on weight predictions. However, after removing the EMG equipment, participants in the “active” group failed to scale their predictive load forces in the same manner as those in the “passive” condition when lifting a novel block. We conclude that cognitive information may play a role in credit assignment, influencing weight prediction when lifting novel objects.

[1]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.

[2]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[3]  H. Ross,et al.  Sensorimotor mechanisms in weight discrimination , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.

[5]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[6]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Transformations: Consolidation, Interference, and Forgetting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  N. Durlach,et al.  Manual discrimination of force using active finger motion , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Zwislocki,et al.  Absolute scaling of sensory magnitudes: A validation , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  R. Johansson,et al.  Integration of sensory information during the programming of precision grip: comments on the contributions of size cues , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Principles of sensorimotor learning , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Lee A Baugh,et al.  Material evidence: interaction of well-learned priors and sensorimotor memory when lifting objects. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  G. Buckingham Getting a grip on heaviness perception: a review of weight illusions and their probable causes , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Observing object lifting errors modulates cortico-spinal excitability and improves object lifting performance , 2014, Cortex.

[18]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  R. Johansson,et al.  Experience Can Change Distinct Size-Weight Priors Engaged in Lifting Objects and Judging their Weights , 2008, Current Biology.

[20]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size–weight illusion , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Two waves of a long-lasting aftereffect of prism adaptation measured over 7 days , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Sensory-motor coordination during grasping and manipulative actions , 1992, Current Biology.

[23]  Olivier White,et al.  Use-Dependent and Error-Based Learning of Motor Behaviors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Enhancing Visuomotor Adaptation by Reducing Error Signals: Single-step (Aware) versus Multiple-step (Unaware) Exposure to Wedge Prisms , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Kelene Fercho,et al.  It’s too quick to blame myself—the effects of fast and slow rates of change on credit assignment during object lifting , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[26]  R. Johansson,et al.  Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor control and learning in altered dynamic environments , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[28]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  K. Newell Motor skill acquisition. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[30]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Persistence of motor memories reflects statistics of the learning event. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[33]  L. Jones,et al.  Perception of force and weight: theory and research. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  Jonathan S. Cant,et al.  Living in a material world: how visual cues to material properties affect the way that we lift objects and perceive their weight. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Lifting without seeing: the role of vision in perceiving and acting upon the size‐weight illusion , 2010 .

[36]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Reach adaptation: what determines whether we learn an internal model of the tool or adapt the model of our arm? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  The size of the visual size cue used for programming manipulative forces during precision grip , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Miles C. Bowman,et al.  Control strategies in object manipulation tasks , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[41]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Dissociable effects of the implicit and explicit memory systems on learning control of reaching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.