Cognitive attribution of the source of an error in object-lifting results in differences in motor generalization
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Vincent S. Huang,et al. Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.
[2] R. Shadmehr,et al. Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.
[3] H. Ross,et al. Sensorimotor mechanisms in weight discrimination , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[4] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.
[5] D. Pélisson,et al. Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[6] J. Krakauer,et al. Adaptation to Visuomotor Transformations: Consolidation, Interference, and Forgetting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[7] J R Flanagan,et al. The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[8] N. Durlach,et al. Manual discrimination of force using active finger motion , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[9] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[10] J. Zwislocki,et al. Absolute scaling of sensory magnitudes: A validation , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] R. Johansson,et al. Integration of sensory information during the programming of precision grip: comments on the contributions of size cues , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[12] G. Stelmach,et al. Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] D. Wolpert,et al. Principles of sensorimotor learning , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[14] Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al. Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] Lee A Baugh,et al. Material evidence: interaction of well-learned priors and sensorimotor memory when lifting objects. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[16] G. Buckingham. Getting a grip on heaviness perception: a review of weight illusions and their probable causes , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. Observing object lifting errors modulates cortico-spinal excitability and improves object lifting performance , 2014, Cortex.
[18] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] R. Johansson,et al. Experience Can Change Distinct Size-Weight Priors Engaged in Lifting Objects and Judging their Weights , 2008, Current Biology.
[20] J. Flanagan,et al. Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size–weight illusion , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[21] Y. Rossetti,et al. Two waves of a long-lasting aftereffect of prism adaptation measured over 7 days , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] K. J. Cole,et al. Sensory-motor coordination during grasping and manipulative actions , 1992, Current Biology.
[23] Olivier White,et al. Use-Dependent and Error-Based Learning of Motor Behaviors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] Yves Rossetti,et al. Enhancing Visuomotor Adaptation by Reducing Error Signals: Single-step (Aware) versus Multiple-step (Unaware) Exposure to Wedge Prisms , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[25] Kelene Fercho,et al. It’s too quick to blame myself—the effects of fast and slow rates of change on credit assignment during object lifting , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[26] R. Johansson,et al. Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[27] J. Lackner,et al. Motor control and learning in altered dynamic environments , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[28] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[29] K. Newell. Motor skill acquisition. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.
[30] K. J. Cole,et al. Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[31] Vincent S. Huang,et al. Persistence of motor memories reflects statistics of the learning event. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[32] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.
[33] L. Jones,et al. Perception of force and weight: theory and research. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.
[34] Jonathan S. Cant,et al. Living in a material world: how visual cues to material properties affect the way that we lift objects and perceive their weight. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. Lifting without seeing: the role of vision in perceiving and acting upon the size‐weight illusion , 2010 .
[36] Jörn Diedrichsen,et al. Reach adaptation: what determines whether we learn an internal model of the tool or adapt the model of our arm? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[37] J. Flanagan,et al. Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[38] M. Mon-Williams,et al. The size of the visual size cue used for programming manipulative forces during precision grip , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[39] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[40] Miles C. Bowman,et al. Control strategies in object manipulation tasks , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[41] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Dissociable effects of the implicit and explicit memory systems on learning control of reaching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.