Musical pitch classes have rainbow hues in pitch class-color synesthesia

Synesthesia, an anomalous blending of senses in which stimulation of one sensory modality produces sensation in a different modality, provides a unique opportunity to study how multimodal information is represented in the human brain. We investigated how pitch classes (do, re, mi, etc.) are associated with the three dimensions of color (hue, saturation, and value/brightness) in 15 subjects who possessed “pitch class-color synesthesia”. Across-subject averaging of reported colors revealed that pitch classes have rainbow hues, beginning with do-red, re-yellow, and so forth, ending with si-violet, accompanied by a decrease in saturation. Enharmonic pitch classes that referred to the same pitch class with a different name produced color sensations according to the name of the base pitch class, e.g., a reddish color for do-sharp and a yellowish color for re-flat. Thus the main factor producing color sensations was the name, not the sound, of the note; behavioral experiments corroborated this interpretation. Pitch class-color synesthesia represents a newly described type of synesthesia that is distinct from the well-known crossmodal association between pitch height and value/brightness. Findings suggest that the two dimensions of musical pitch, pitch class and pitch height, are mapped to the hue-saturation plane and the value/brightness dimension of color, respectively.

[1]  W. Singer,et al.  The Merit of Synesthesia for Consciousness Research , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[2]  T L Hubbard,et al.  Synesthesia-like mappings of lightness, pitch, and melodic interval. , 1996, The American journal of psychology.

[3]  R. Shepard Circularity in Judgments of Relative Pitch , 1964 .

[4]  R. Racine,et al.  Pitch interactions in the perception of isolated musical triads , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  David Allen,et al.  Octave discriminability of musical and non-musical subjects , 1967 .

[6]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Non-random associations of graphemes to colours in synaesthetic and non-synaesthetic populations , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[7]  D. Ausubel,et al.  Synesthesia in children: association of colors with pure tone frequencies. , 1956, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[8]  A. H. Takeuchi,et al.  Absolute pitch. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Seeing double: The role of meaning in alphanumeric-colour synaesthesia , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[10]  L E Marks,et al.  On associations of light and sound: the mediation of brightness, pitch, and loudness. , 1974, The American journal of psychology.

[11]  Matthew Rizzo,et al.  Synesthesia , 1989, Neurology.

[12]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  Ken'ichi Miyazaki,et al.  Learning Absolute Pitch by Children: A Cross-Sectional Study , 2006 .

[14]  Tsutomu Nakada,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of absolute pitch and relative pitch. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  D. Eagleman,et al.  Wednesday Is Indigo Blue: Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia , 2009 .

[16]  Richard E. Cytowic Synesthesia: A union of the senses, 2nd ed. , 2002 .

[17]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Sound-Colour Synaesthesia: to What Extent Does it Use Cross-Modal Mechanisms Common to us All? , 2006, Cortex.

[18]  L. Marks On colored-hearing synesthesia: cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.