Stress-induced hyperthermia in the rat: comparison of classical and novel recording methods

Stress causes a rise in body temperature in laboratory animals (stress-induced hyperthermia). However, the direct effect of common stressors in animal research, i.e. transportation between holding and test rooms or isolation of animals, on body temperature has not been investigated to its full extent. To address this question, it is important to have a reliable and simple monitoring technique, which does not induce stress itself. In the present study, we investigated stress-related changes in body temperature of F344/Hw rats after (1) moving the cage within the holding room, (2) moving the cage from the holding room to another test room and (3) social deprivation (isolation). A combination of two different body temperature recording methods was used to clarify their accuracy and stress-inductive character: rectal temperature recording and peritoneal implanted temperature sensors (Thermochron iButtons). The results demonstrate that (1) different stressors induce a significant rise in body temperature, (2) which is detectable for more than 60 min and (3) it is of importance to standardize temperature recording methods in order to avoid confounding effects of the recording method itself. Furthermore, Thermochron iButtons are more accurate and reliable for body temperature studies than rectal recordings.

[1]  N. Murakami,et al.  Effect of a central CRF antagonist on cardiovascular and thermoregulatory responses induced by stress or IL-1 beta. , 1993, The American journal of physiology.

[2]  Stephan von Hörsten,et al.  Factors influencing behavior of group-housed male rats in the social interaction test Focus on cohort removal , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  B. Olivier,et al.  Stress-induced hyperthermia in mice: A methodological study , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[4]  C. Suaudeau,et al.  Regulation of body temperature and nocipception induced by non-noxious stress in rat , 1984, Brain Research.

[5]  S. Levine Infantile experience and resistance to physiological stress. , 1957, Science.

[6]  J. Stephenson,et al.  Core temperature: Some shortcomings of rectal temperature measurements , 1977, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  M. Verleye,et al.  Effects of etifoxine on stress-induced hyperthermia, freezing behavior and colonic motor activation in rats , 2004, Physiology & Behavior.

[8]  A. Vander,et al.  Stress-induced rise of body temperature in rats is the same in warm and cool environments , 1990, Physiology & Behavior.

[9]  P. Rehbinder,et al.  Recommendations for the health monitoring of rodent and rabbit colonies in breeding and experimental units , 2002, Laboratory animals.

[10]  R. Eikelboom Learned anticipatory rise in body temperature due to handling , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[11]  Berend Olivier,et al.  Stress-Induced Hyperthermia in Singly Housed Mice , 1997, Physiology & Behavior.

[12]  T. Hori,et al.  Mechanisms and Mediators of Psychological Stress-Induced Rise in Core Temperature , 2001, Psychosomatic medicine.

[13]  J. Grayson,et al.  Observations on temperature distribution and liver blood flow in the rat , 1952, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  A. Herz,et al.  Involvement of endorphins in emotional hyperthermia of rats. , 1978, Life sciences.

[15]  P. Lomax Measurement of ‘Core’ Temperature in the Rat , 1966, Nature.

[16]  E. Briese,et al.  Colonic temperature of rats during handling. , 1970, Acta physiologica latino americana.

[17]  J. Gugten,et al.  Stress-induced hyperthermia in mice: Hormonal correlates , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[18]  S. Daan,et al.  Stress-Induced Changes in Circadian Rhythms of Body Temperature and Activity in Rats Are not Caused by Pacemaker Changes , 1997, Journal of biological rhythms.

[19]  M. Menaker,et al.  Thermochron iButtons: An Inexpensive Method for Long-Term Recording of Core Body Temperature in Untethered Animals , 2003, Journal of biological rhythms.

[20]  A. Lecci,et al.  A model to measure anticipatory anxiety in mice? , 2004, Psychopharmacology.