Dwarf mongoose alarm calls: investigating a complex non-human animal call

Communication plays a vital role in the social lives of many species and varies greatly in complexity. One possible way to increase communicative complexity is by combining signals into longer sequences, which has been proposed as a mechanism allowing species with a limited repertoire to increase their communicative output. In mammals, most studies on combinatoriality have focused on vocal communication in non-human primates. Here, we investigated a potential combination of alarm calls in the dwarf mongoose (Helogale parvula), a non-primate mammal. Acoustic analyses and playback experiments with a wild population suggest: (i) that dwarf mongooses produce a complex call type (T3) which, at least at the surface level, seems to comprise units that are not functionally different to two meaningful alarm calls (aerial and terrestrial); and (ii) that this T3 call functions as a general alarm, produced in response to a wide range of threats. Using a novel approach, we further explored multiple interpretations of the T3 call based on the information content of the apparent comprising calls and how they are combined. We also considered an alternative, non-combinatorial interpretation that frames T3 as the origin, rather than the product, of the individual alarm calls. This study complements previous knowledge of vocal combinatoriality in non-primate mammals and introduces an approach that could facilitate comparisons between different animal and human communication systems.

[1]  R. Byrne,et al.  Titi monkey call sequences vary with predator location and type , 2013, Biology Letters.

[2]  M. Manser,et al.  Wild dwarf mongooses produce general alert and predator-specific alarm calls , 2017 .

[3]  Tamar Flash,et al.  Motor primitives in vertebrates and invertebrates , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[6]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Suffixation influences receivers' behaviour in non-human primates , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  Marta B Manser,et al.  Moving calls: a vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Campbell's Monkeys Use Affixation to Alter Call Meaning , 2009, PloS one.

[9]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  The Syntax and Meaning of Wild Gibbon Songs , 2006, PloS one.

[10]  Xin-Yuan Song,et al.  Evaluation of the Bayesian and Maximum Likelihood Approaches in Analyzing Structural Equation Models with Small Sample Sizes , 2004, Multivariate behavioral research.

[11]  W. Strange Evolution of language. , 1984, JAMA.

[12]  A. Radford,et al.  Call of duty? Variation in use of the watchman's song by sentinel dwarf mongooses, Helogale parvula , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  D. Kroodsma,et al.  Female mate choice and songbird song repertoires , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  Alban Lemasson,et al.  Sex differences in the vocal repertoire of adult red‐capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus): a multi‐level acoustic analysis , 2010, American journal of primatology.

[15]  Martin A. Nowak,et al.  The evolution of syntactic communication , 2000, Nature.

[16]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Context-related call combinations in female Diana monkeys , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[17]  Roger Mundry,et al.  Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  O. Rasa The Ethology and Sociology of the Dwarf Mongoose (Helogale undulata rufula) , 1977, Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie.

[19]  M A Nowak,et al.  Evolutionary biology of language. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[20]  Philippe Schlenker,et al.  Formal monkey linguistics , 2016 .

[21]  François Christophe Egidio Pellegrino,et al.  Across-Language Perspective on Speech Information Rate , 2011 .

[22]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[23]  D. Blumstein Golden-marmot alarm calls. I: The production of situationally specific vocalizations , 1995 .

[24]  M A Nowak,et al.  An error limit for the evolution of language , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  R. Jackendoff Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[27]  Julie Gros-Louis,et al.  Alarm calls of white-faced capuchin monkeys: an acoustic analysis , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Animal syntax , 2019, Current Biology.

[29]  A. P. Allen,et al.  Concatenation of ‘alert’ and ‘identity’ segments in dingoes’ alarm calls , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[30]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  F. Huddle Coordination , 1966, Open Knowledge Institutions.

[32]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Call combinations in monkeys: Compositional or idiomatic expressions? , 2012, Brain and Language.

[33]  J. Sim,et al.  The kappa statistic in reliability studies: use, interpretation, and sample size requirements. , 2005, Physical therapy.

[34]  M. Manser,et al.  Vocal Complexity in Meerkats and Other Mongoose Species , 2014 .

[35]  Mark E. Laidre,et al.  Principles of Animal Communication Principles of Animal Communication, 2nd edn. By JACK W. Bradbury & Sandra L. Vehrencamp. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer (2011). Pp. xiv + 697. Price $99.95. , 2012, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  R. Byrne,et al.  The alarm call system of wild black-fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus nigrifrons , 2012, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[37]  William N. Venables,et al.  Modern Applied Statistics with S , 2010 .

[38]  Balthasar Bickel,et al.  Language evolution: syntax before phonology? , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  Serge A. Wich,et al.  Acoustic Properties of Long Calls Given by Flanged Male Orang-Utans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) Reflect Both Individual Identity and Context , 2010 .

[40]  O. Rasa,et al.  Coordinated Vigilance in Dwarf Mongoose Family Groups: The ‘Watchman's Song’ Hypothesis and the Costs of Guarding , 2010 .

[41]  S. Creel,et al.  Subordinate reproduction in dwarf mongooses , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  Sabrina Engesser,et al.  Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  William M. Fields,et al.  The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. , 2000 .

[44]  J. Carlin,et al.  Bias, prevalence and kappa. , 1993, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[45]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Bonobos Extract Meaning from Call Sequences , 2011, PloS one.

[46]  M. Manser,et al.  Segmental concatenation of individual signatures and context cues in banded mongoose (Mungos mungo) close calls , 2012, BMC Biology.

[47]  C. Boesch,et al.  Call combinations in wild chimpanzees , 2005 .

[48]  S. Pika Unpeeling the layers of communicative complexity , 2017, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  W. W. Muir,et al.  Regression Diagnostics: Identifying Influential Data and Sources of Collinearity , 1980 .

[50]  Toshitaka N Suzuki,et al.  Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls , 2016, Nature Communications.

[51]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls , 2006, Nature.

[52]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[53]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.