Choline Uptake in the Frontal Cortex Is Proportional to the Absolute Error of a Temporal Memory Translation Constant in Mature and Aged Rats

Abstract The relationship between the magnitude of the error in the content of temporal memory and sodium-dependent high-affinity choline uptake (SDHACU) in the frontal cortex and hippocampus was examined in mature (10- to 16-month-old) and aged (24- to 30-month-old) male rats. The peak time of the response rate distribution that relates the probability of a response to signal duration in a 20-s peak-interval timing procedure was used to index the remembered time of reinforcement. Regression analyses indicated that SDHACU in the frontal cortex of both mature and aged rats and in the hippocampus of aged rats is proportional to the absolute error in the content of temporal memory. These biochemical effects of peak-interval training were also compared with biochemical measures taken from control rats that received random-interval training. This comparison indicated that the observed changes in SDHACU were dependent upon the predictability of the programmed time of reinforcement and age-related changes in memory encoding and retrieval.

[1]  R. Church,et al.  Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials. , 1994 .

[2]  W. Meck,et al.  Hippocampal function is required for feedback control of an internal clock's criterion. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  C. A. Marsan,et al.  Neuronal plasticity and memory formation , 1982 .

[4]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[5]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[6]  W H Meck,et al.  The 'internal clocks' of circadian and interval timing. , 1997, Endeavour.

[7]  T. Durkin,et al.  Septo-hippocampal and nBM-cortical cholinergic neurones exhibit differential time-courses of activation as a function of both type and duration of spatial memory testing in mice , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  W. Meck,et al.  Simultaneous temporal processing is sensitive to prenatal choline availability in mature and aged rats , 1997, Neuroreport.

[9]  L. Kamin Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning , 1967 .

[10]  A. R. McIntosh,et al.  Recruitment of unique neural systems to support visual memory in normal aging , 1999, Current Biology.

[11]  R. Church,et al.  Arginine vasopressin innoculates against age-related increases in sodium-dependent high affinity choline uptake and discrepancies in the content of temporal memory. , 1986, European journal of pharmacology.

[12]  W. Meck Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[13]  W. Meck Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[14]  W H Meck,et al.  Timing for the absence of a stimulus: the gap paradigm reversed. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[15]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[16]  R. Church,et al.  Cholinergic modulation of the content of temporal memory. , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  M. Kuhar,et al.  ON THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN [3H]CHOLINE UPTAKE ACTIVATION AND [3H]ACETYLCHOLINE RELEASE , 1977, Journal of neurochemistry.

[18]  D. Olton,et al.  Behavior alters the uptake of [3h]choline into acetylcholinergic neurons of the nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area , 1984, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  M. Kuhar,et al.  HIGH AFFINITY CHOLINE UPTAKE: IONIC AND ENERGY REQUIREMENTS , 1976, Journal of neurochemistry.

[20]  Michela Gallagher,et al.  An age-related spatial learning deficit: Choline uptake distinguishes “impaired” and “unimpaired” rats , 1988, Neurobiology of Aging.

[21]  J. Contrera,et al.  CHOLINE: HIGH AFFINITY UPTAKE IN VIVO BY RAT HIPPOCAMPUS , 1977, Journal of neurochemistry.

[22]  M. Kuhar,et al.  SODIUM‐DEPENDENT HIGH AFFINITY CHOLINE UPTAKE: A REGULATORY STEP IN THE SYNTHESIS OF ACETYLCHOLINE , 1976, Journal of neurochemistry.

[23]  B. Campbell,et al.  Punishment and aversive behavior , 1969 .

[24]  W. Meck Vasopressin metabolite neuropeptide facilitates simultaneous temporal processing , 1987, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  D. Olton,et al.  Neurons in the lateral agranular frontal cortex have divided attention correlates in a simultaneous temporal processing task , 2001, Neuroscience.

[26]  W. Meck,et al.  Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. , 2000, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[27]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Information processing in animals : memory mechanisms , 1983 .

[28]  T G Turkington,et al.  Adult age differences in the functional neuroanatomy of verbal recognition memory , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[29]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  H. Terrace,et al.  Autoshaping and Conditioning Theory , 1980 .

[31]  O. H. Lowry,et al.  Protein measurement with the Folin phenol reagent. , 1951, The Journal of biological chemistry.

[32]  W H Meck,et al.  Paying Attention to Time as one Gets Older , 2001, Psychological science.

[33]  J. Danks,et al.  Animal Cognition and Sequential Behavior , 2002, Springer US.

[34]  D. Olton,et al.  Cholinergic and GABAergic modulation of medial septal area: effect on working memory. , 1990, Behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  R. Church,et al.  Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by simultaneous temporal processing , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  W H Meck,et al.  The 'internal clocks' of circadian and interval timing. , 1997, Endeavour.

[37]  E. Tulving,et al.  Age‐related differences in effective neural connectivity during encoding and recall , 1997, Neuroreport.

[38]  R. Church,et al.  Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[39]  H. Rommelspacher,et al.  Changes in high affinity choline uptake in behavioral experiments. , 1978, Life sciences.

[40]  M. Kuhar,et al.  Impulse-flow regulation of high affinity choline uptake in brain cholinergic nerve terminals , 1975, Nature.

[41]  R. Church,et al.  Representation of time , 1990, Cognition.

[42]  W. Meck,et al.  Dissecting the Brain's Internal Clock: How Frontal–Striatal Circuitry Keeps Time and Shifts Attention , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[43]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[44]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Effects of training on a spatial memory task on high affinity choline uptake in hippocampus and cortex in young adult and aged rats , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[45]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[46]  R. Church,et al.  Separation of hippocampal and amygdaloid involvement in temporal memory dysfunctions , 1987, Brain Research.

[47]  M. Kuhar,et al.  Utilization of sodium-dependent high affinity choline uptake in vitro as a measure of the activity of cholinergic neurons in vivo. , 1975, Life sciences.