Knowledge Distribution and the Effect of Design Tools on the Design Process

This paper compares the cognitive performance of architecture students when designing tasks using one of the three design tools: pencil and paper, software Sketch Up and Rhinoceros 3D. It questions if a design tool can affect when knowledge is generated and used in the duration of design activity. This is explored through a protocol ‘think aloud’ study for which a new coding scheme was developed. The methodology is grounded on the theory of Distributed Cognition and Zhang and Norman’s (Cognit Sci 18(1):87–122, 1994) method of ‘representational analysis’, based on which, knowledge is either ‘internal’ in that it is actively memorized by the designer or is ‘external’ in that it is implicitly made available via a stimuli like a design tool. Using an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) test, for the five participants of this study, external knowledge generated significantly earlier on within the process when using Sketch Up compared to the other two tools.

[1]  John S. Gero,et al.  Roles of knowledge while designing and their implications for CAAD , 2001, CAADRIA proceedings.

[2]  Alva Noë,et al.  Action in Perception , 2006, Representation and Mind.

[3]  Terry Dartnall,et al.  Does the World Leak Into the Mind? Active Externalism, "Internalism" and Epistemology , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  J. Poon,et al.  Co-evolution of the fitness function and design solution for design exploration , 1995, Proceedings of 1995 IEEE International Conference on Evolutionary Computation.

[5]  P. Langley,et al.  A structure for representing problem formulation in design , 2011 .

[6]  James D. Hollan,et al.  Distributed cognition: toward a new foundation for human-computer interaction research , 2000, TCHI.

[7]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data , 1984 .

[8]  John S. Gero,et al.  To sketch or not to sketch? That is the question , 2006 .

[9]  Renate Eisentraut Styles of problem solving and their influence on the design process , 1999 .

[10]  Richard Coyne,et al.  Innovation and Creativity in Knowledge-based CAD , 1987 .

[11]  Vinod Goel,et al.  The Structure of Design Problem Spaces , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  B. Nijstad,et al.  Production blocking and idea generation: Does blocking interfere with cognitive processes? , 2003 .

[13]  P. Tibbetts :Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind , 2009 .

[14]  Rudolf Arnheim The dynamics of architectural form : based on the 1975 Mary Duke Biddle lectures at the Cooper Union , 1977 .

[15]  John S. Gero,et al.  A computational framework for concept formation for a situated design agent , 2000, Knowl. Based Syst..

[16]  John S. Gero,et al.  Impact of Using Rule Algorithms on Designers' Behavior in a Parametric Design Environment: Preliminary Result from a Pilot Study , 2013, CAAD Futures.

[17]  Masaki Suwa,et al.  Macroscopic analysis of design processes based on a scheme for coding designers' cognitive actions , 1998 .

[18]  Ben Jonson,et al.  Design ideation: the conceptual sketch in the digital age , 2005 .

[19]  Jim McLennan,et al.  Effects of Concurrent Verbalization on a Time-Critical, Dynamic Decision-Making Task , 2000, The Journal of general psychology.

[20]  Nigel Cross,et al.  Creativity in the design process: co-evolution of problem–solution , 2001 .

[21]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.

[22]  William J. Mitchell,et al.  The Logic of Architecture: Design, Computation, and Cognition , 1992 .

[23]  Jiajie Zhang,et al.  Representations in Distributed Cognitive Tasks , 1994, Cogn. Sci..

[24]  Chris Baber,et al.  Tool use as distributed cognition: how tools help, hinder and define manual skill , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[25]  Nienke Meulman,et al.  An ERP study on L2 syntax processing: When do learners fail? , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[26]  A. Clark,et al.  The Extended Mind , 1998, Analysis.