Evidence for the auditory P3a reflecting an automatic process: Elicitation during highly-focused continuous visual attention

The P3a is an event-related potential (ERP) component believed to reflect an attention-switch to task-irrelevant stimuli or stimulus information. The present study concerns the automaticity of the processes underlying the auditory P3a. More specifically, we investigated whether the auditory P3a is an attention-independent component, that is, whether it can still be elicited under highly-focused selective attention to a different (visual) channel. Furthermore, we examined whether the auditory P3a can be modulated by the demands of the visual diversion task. Subjects performed a continuous visual tracking task that varied in difficulty, based on the number of objects to-be-tracked. Task-irrelevant auditory stimuli were presented at very rapid and random rates concurrently to the visual task. The auditory sequence included rare increments (+10 dB) and decrements (-20 dB) in intensity relative to the frequently-presented standard stimulus. Importantly, the auditory deviant stimuli elicited a significant P3a during the most difficult visual task, when conditions were optimised to prevent attentional slippage to the auditory channel. This finding suggests that the elicitation of the auditory P3a does not require available central capacity, and confirms the automatic nature of the processes underlying this ERP component. Moreover, the difficulty of the visual task did not modulate either the mismatch negativity (MMN) or the P3a but did have an effect on a late (350-400 ms) negativity, an ERP deflection perhaps related to a subsequent evaluation of the auditory change. Together, these results imply that the auditory P3a could reflect a strongly-automatic process, one that does not require and is not modulated by attention.

[1]  A. Dittmann-Balçar,et al.  Attention-dependent allocation of auditory processing resources as measured by mismatch negativity. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[2]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Working memory controls involuntary attention switching: evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  John Polich,et al.  P3a from auditory white noise stimuli , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[4]  K. Arnell,et al.  The attentional blink across stimulus modalities: Evidence for central processing limitations. , 1999 .

[5]  E. Schröger,et al.  Bottom-up influences on working memory: behavioral and electrophysiological distraction varies with distractor strength. , 2004, Experimental psychology.

[6]  C. Escera,et al.  Electrical responses reveal the temporal dynamics of brain events during involuntary attention switching , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  I. Winkler,et al.  Involuntary Attention and Distractibility as Evaluated with Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2000, Audiology and Neurotology.

[8]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Automatic and attention-dependent processing of auditory stimulus information , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  T Fernández,et al.  Primary task demands modulate P3a amplitude. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[10]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Electrical Signs of Selective Attention in the Human Brain , 1973, Science.

[11]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Effects of stimulation rate and attribute cuing on event-related potentials during selective auditory attention. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[12]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Attentional influence on the mismatch negativity , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  E. Schröger,et al.  Two separate mechanisms underlie auditory change detection and involuntary control of attention , 2006, Brain Research.

[14]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Varieties of attention , 1984 .

[15]  L. Schiebinger,et al.  Commentary on Risto Naatanen (1990). The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive fenctiono BBS 13s201-2888 , 1991 .

[16]  T. Picton,et al.  The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[17]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[18]  Stefan Berti,et al.  Examining task-dependencies of different attentional processes as reflected in the P3a and reorienting negativity components of the human event-related brain potential , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Processing of novel sounds and frequency changes in the human auditory cortex: magnetoencephalographic recordings. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[20]  E. Schröger,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction , 2007, Brain Research.

[21]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[22]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Auditory distraction by duration and location deviants: a behavioral and event-related potential study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  K. Forster,et al.  Forty-five years after Broadbent (1958): still no identification without attention. , 2004, Psychological review.

[24]  J. Polich,et al.  P3a, perceptual distinctiveness, and stimulus modality. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[25]  S. Hackley,et al.  An evaluation of the automaticity of sensory processing using event-related potentials and brain-stem reflexes. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[26]  E. Schröger,et al.  Attentional orienting and reorienting is indicated by human event‐related brain potentials , 1998, Neuroreport.

[27]  A. Muller-Gass,et al.  The effect of visual task difficulty and attentional direction on the detection of acoustic change as indexed by the Mismatch Negativity , 2006, Brain Research.

[28]  E. Schröger,et al.  Preattentive Memory-Based Comparison of Sound Intensity , 2003, Audiology and Neurotology.

[29]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[30]  R. Näätänen The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[31]  Norbert Kathmann,et al.  Stability of the mismatch negativity under different stimulus and attention conditions , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[32]  J. C. Woestenburg,et al.  The removal of the eye-movement artifact from the EEG by regression analysis in the frequency domain , 1983, Biological Psychology.

[33]  I. Winkler,et al.  Memory-based or afferent processes in mismatch negativity (MMN): a review of the evidence. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[34]  C Alain,et al.  Effects of visual attentional load on auditory processing , 2000, Neuroreport.

[35]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Intermodal selective attention. II. Effects of attentional load on processing of auditory and visual stimuli in central space. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[36]  Aysenil Belger,et al.  Visual task complexity modulates the brain's response to unattended auditory novelty , 2005, Neuroreport.

[37]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[38]  Peng Yuan,et al.  The effect of visuospatial attentional load on the processing of irrelevant acoustic distractors , 2006, NeuroImage.

[39]  K Alho,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms underlying orienting of attention towards auditory frequency changes , 2001, Neuroreport.

[40]  F. Bloom,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic recordings demonstrate attentional modulation of mismatch-related neural activity in human auditory cortex. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[41]  E. Schröger,et al.  A neural mechanism for involuntary attention shifts to changes in auditory stimulation. , 1996, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.

[42]  E. Donchin,et al.  The influence of stimulus deviance and novelty on the P300 and novelty P3. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[43]  R. Knight,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Involuntary Attention to Acoustic Novelty and Change , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  D. Woods,et al.  Auditory selective attention in middle-aged and elderly subjects: an event-related brain potential study. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.